A Multimodal Analysis - English | Học viện Hàng Không Việt Nam

A Multimodal Analysis - English | Học viện Hàng Không Việt Nam được sưu tầm và soạn thảo dưới dạng file PDF để gửi tới các bạn sinh viên cùng tham khảo, ôn tập đầy đủ kiến thức, chuẩn bị cho các buổi học thật tốt. Mời bạn đọc đón xem!

Môn:
Trường:

Học viện Hàng Không Việt Nam 556 tài liệu

Thông tin:
9 trang 5 tháng trước

Bình luận

Vui lòng đăng nhập hoặc đăng ký để gửi bình luận.

A Multimodal Analysis - English | Học viện Hàng Không Việt Nam

A Multimodal Analysis - English | Học viện Hàng Không Việt Nam được sưu tầm và soạn thảo dưới dạng file PDF để gửi tới các bạn sinh viên cùng tham khảo, ôn tập đầy đủ kiến thức, chuẩn bị cho các buổi học thật tốt. Mời bạn đọc đón xem!

28 14 lượt tải Tải xuống
1. Introduction
Mathematic Discourse (MD) is referred to as
multisemiotic as it is constructed from more than
one semiotic resource - language, visual images
and mathematical symbolism (O’Halloran
2004, p.21). The view of mathematics as a
multisemiotic discourse is significant in a
pedagogical context as a better understanding
of the functions of mathematical symbolism and
visual images permits a re-evaluation of the role
of language in the construction of meaning in
this naturalized domain. Such an understanding
proves to be even more essential in the case of
content and language integrated learning (CLIL)
in a foreign context, where the learners have to
cope with both mathematic problems per se and
a foreign language.
This study is an attempt to investigate MD
written in English for primary school learners.
*
Tel.: 84-905242270
Email: tnmynhat70@gmail.com
Specifically, the present study examines the
following research questions: (1) To what
extent is each of the three semiotic resources
represented in the materials of learning
mathematics in English (ME) developed for
young learners (YLs)? and (2) How many
words do YLs need to know to understand
the vocabulary in ME and to what extent can
these materials enhance incidental vocabulary
learning? Two major areas of interest are the
lexis specific to the field of Mathematics and
that to children’s everyday world.
2. Mathematical discourse
O’Halloran’s (2004) study can be best
viewed as a first step towards a comprehensive
Systemic-functional Grammar for MD.
The major concern of this study is to
investigate the multisemiotic nature of MD.
She developed theoretical frameworks for
mathematical symbolism and visual display.
A MULTIMODAL ANALYSIS OF MATHEMATICAL
DISCOURSE IN ENGLISH FOR YOUNG LEARNERS
Ton Nu My Nhat
*
Department of Foreign Languages, Quy Nhon University
170 An Duong Vuong, Quy Nhon, Binh Dinh, Vietnam
Received 09 October 2017
Revised 02 November 2017; Accepted 27 November 2017
Abstract: Of multiple discourses where the Vietnamese young learners are increasingly engaged to
develop their English proficiency, English mathematical discourse (MD) has proved to be more and more
popular. This paper explores the materials in this realm from multisemiotic perspective. In particular, it
deals with two questions: (1) to what extent each of the three semiotic resources - language, visual images
and mathematical symbolism - is represented in the materials of learning mathematics in English (ME)
developed for young learners (YL) and (2) how many words the YLs need to know to comprehend the
language component of these materials. Data for illustrations and discussions are withdrawn from the
printed resources currently accessible in the Vietnamese context. The results offer insights into the functions
of other resources in constructing meanings apart from the well-established role of language as well as the
vocabulary load of these materials. The paper concludes with a discussion of pedagogical significance of
this study for material designers, teachers and learners and implications for further research.
Keywords: mathematical discourse, multisemiotic discourse, high frequency word list
T.N.M. Nhat / VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-10194
As reviewed in O’Halloran’s (2004, pp.
13-15) the multisemiotic approach, where
language, visual images and mathematical
symbolism are considered semiotic resources,
originally stems from O’Toole’s (1994, 1995,
1999) extensions of Halliday’s (1978, 1994)
Systemic-funtional approach to displayed art,
and Lemke’s (1998, 2000, 2003) early work
in mathematical and scientific discourse.
Following are the central tenets which are
relevant to the present study.
(1) MD is considered as ‘multisemiotic’
construction; that is, discourses formed
through choices from the functional sign
systems of language, mathematical symbolism
and visual display.
(2) MD involves language, mathematical
symbolism and visual images. The functions
of each semiotic resource may be summarized
as follows. Patterns of relations are encoded
and rearranged symbolically for the solution to
the problem. Due to the limited functionality
of the symbolism, language functions as
the meta-discourse to contextualize the
problem, to explain the activity sequence
which is undertaken for the solution to the
mathematics problem. Visual images in
the form of abstract and statistical graphs,
geometrical diagrams, and other types of
diagrams and forms of visual display, mirror
our perceptual understanding of the world,
showing the relations in a multi-dimensional
spatio-temporal format. They thus connect
and extend common-sense experience to the
mathematical symbolic descriptions.
(3) MD depends on both intrasemiosis and
intersemiosis. As the types of meaning made
by each semiotic resource are fundamentally
different (p.16), and thus the three semiotic
resources fulfil individual functions, the success
of mathematics depends on utilizing and
combining the unique meaning potentials of
language, symbolism and visual display in such
a way that the semantic expansion is greater than
the sum of meanings derived from each of the
three resources. Intersemiosis refers to meaning
which arises from the relations and shifts across
the three semiotic resources; Intrasemiosis
is meaning within one semiotic resource.
Royce (1998, p. 26, cited in O’Halloran, 2004:
159) refers to intersemiosis as intersemiotic
complementarity where visual and verbal
modes semantically complement each other
to produce a single textual phenomenon’. As
Royce and Lemke (1998, cited in O’Halloran
2004, p. 159) explain, the product is synergistic
or in that the result is greater multiplicative
than the sum of the parts.
Language, symbolism and visual
images function together in
mathematical discourse to create
a semantic circuit which permits
semantic expansions beyond that
possible through the sum of the
three resources. Following this
view, the success of mathematics
as a discourse stems from the fact
that it draws upon the meaning
potentials of language, visual
images and the symbolism in
very specific ways. That is, the
discourse, grammar and display
systems for each resource have
evolved to function as interlocking
system networks rather than
isolated phenomena. (O’Halloran
2004: 159)
(4) Mathematical printed texts are
typically organized in very specific ways
which simultaneously permit segration and
integration of the three semiotic resources (p.
11). The systems of meaning for language,
symbolism and visual images are integrated
in such a way that the behaviour of physical
systems may be described. Choices from the
three semiotic resources function integratively.
That is, the linguistic text and the graphs
contain symbolic elements and the symbolic
text contains linguistic elements. The symbolic
elements may also be either spatially separated
from the main body of the linguistic text or
embedded within the linguistic text.
VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101 95
3. Methodology
3.1. Materials
The books which served as the data of the
present study comprise two sets. The first set
consists of two books published by Vietnam
Education Publishing House - Math ViOlympic
4 (Đặng Minh Tuấn & Nguyễn Thị Hải, 2016)
and Math ViOlympic 5 (Đặng Minh Tuấn &
Nguyễn Thị Bích Phượng, 2016) the second ;
is two books published by Singapore Asia
Publishers - Tan, A. Learning Maths 1B (
2016a) and (Tan, A. Learning Maths 2A
2016b). Math ViOlympic 4 Math and
ViOlympic 5 are the only two published in
Vietnam so far in this realm. From the series
published by the foreign publisher, these two
books were chosen for analysis as these two
are for the children of the same age groups as
those in the first set. The number of problems
and of running words of the verbal texts in
each book is shown in Table 1.
3.2. Instruments
The sets of materials were analysed
using Compleat Lexical Tutor developed by
Tom Cobb (available at http://www.lextutor).
VocabProfile gives all the information
regarding vocabularies of a text - the number
of type, token, word families, type-token ratio,
function and content words and even breaks any
English text into its frequency levels according
to the thousand-levels scheme, Academic and
off-list words, indicated by colours. Frequency
extracts frequency lists from the corpora.
TextLexCompare is used to tract the amount of
vocabulary repetition across the books within
each set and across the sets.
3.3. Procedures
To achieve the aims, the texts were typed
and computerized. The data was first closely
analyzed in terms of the distribution of the
verbal, visual, and symbolic components.
Whereas the statistics of the linguistic and
symbolic components were computationally
performed, the images were manually
calculated. To analyze the vocabulary of the
books, the raw data were processed to omit the
proper nouns. This is because many researchers
have taken the approach that proper nouns may
be easily understood by readers (e.g. Nation,
2006); how proper nouns are handled makes a
big difference to an output profile (Cobb, 2010).
The symbolic components and numbers, which
are inherent and pervasive of this genre, were
also omitted. The data were then submitted to
the vocabulary profile after being converted to
text files, using the BNC-20 wordlist.
4. Findings and discussion
4.1. Distribution of the three semiotic
resources
As explicated above, the organisation of
mathematical printed texts, typically involving
three semiotic resources, simultaneously
permit segregation and integration of these
componential elements. An in-depth analysis
of the data, both computationally and manually,
yielded insightful findings on the distribution
of the resources, as shown in Table 2.
Table 1. Number of problems and words in individual books analysed
Book No. of Problems Running words
Learning Maths 1B
Learning Maths 2A
Math ViOlympic 4
Math ViOlympic 5
381
393
555
400
3488
1589
5578
5141
Total 1,729 15,796
T.N.M. Nhat / VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-10196
The most noticeable feature is the presence
of all the resources in all the books analysed.
However, whereas the series Learning Maths
tends to favor symbolic and imageries, the
Math ViOlympic series displays an
overwhelming predominance of language. All
the problems in the Math ViOlympic series are
represented via language (100%); by contrast,
images account for less than 10 percent, of
which approximately a half are just for the
illustrative purpose rather than functioning as
an integral component of the problems in
question. In other words, these images can be
omitted without any inhibition to
understanding on the part of the learner.
In the meantime, visuals are always
contextualized in relation to the linguistic
text and/or the symbolic component in the
Learning Maths series. Another significant
finding from the data is the particularly high
proportion of images in , Learning Math 1B
which is likely to result from an awareness
of the meaningful function of this means in
MD in general and its motivating role to YLs
of language in particular. Accordingly, in
this book, the two other resources make up a
mere 7.87% and 5.24%. Finally, the symbolic
component is moderately high in all the three
other books (76.59%, 53.5%, and 43.42%).
This result is obviously due to the function of
this semiotic resource in MD, as described in
the third section.
4.2. Features of the linguistic text
To answer the second research question to
what extent doing mathematics in English can
be beneficial to the YLs’ vocabulary growth ,
the verbal data were submitted to VocabProfile
Frequency, and TextLexCompare. Table 3 and
4 summarize the data in terms of tokens, types,
and families of the two corpora, Learning
Maths and Math ViOlympic, respectively; the
cumulative coverage for each book is shown
in Table 5.
Tables 3 and 4 show that the tokens
spread over the 20 most frequent 1,000
word families of the BNC. The importance
of knowing the most frequent word families
is clearly demonstrated in the first rows of
these three tables. The first 1,000 word
families from the BNC account for up to
approximately four-fifths of tokens in the
problems in all these books 76.29%,
84.02%, 84.06%, and 81.13%. For example,
regarding Math ViOlympic 4, the first row
indicates that 424 different word forms
(types) are the source of these 4689 tokens.
These 424 types reduce to 303 word-
families. Similarly, as for Learning Maths
2A, the first 1,000 word families account for
1335 of the tokens, 225 of the types, and
173 of the families. It is useful to consider
the output in terms of word families because
similarity in forms and meanings for tokens
from the same family may facilitate
understanding and retention. It is also clear
that after the second 1,000 word-families,
the decreasing rate of the tokens tend to be
approximately the same across the four
books. From the third-1,000 onwards, the
numbers of families thin out rapidly, which
Table 2. Distribution of three semiotic resources
Language
Symbolic
elements
Images
Total of
Problems
Illustrative Integral
No. (%) No. (%) No. (%) No. (%)
Learning Maths 1B
Learning Maths 2A
30 (7.87%) 20 (5.24%) 4 (1.04%) 351(92.12%) 381
158 (40.20%) 301(76.59%) 2 (0.50%) 31 (7.88%) 393
Math ViOlympic 4
Math ViOlympic 5
555 (100%) 241 (43.42%) 26 (4.68%) 33 (5.94%) 555
400 (100%) 214 (53.5%) 5 (1.25%) 37 (9.25%) 400
VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101 97
suggests that the number of low frequency
words is few and far between.
As shown in Table 6, it is also important
to note that of these huge coverages of the
first 1,000 word-families, the number of the
function words tends to double that of the
content words throughout the data.
Assuming that proper nouns and
Table 3. Tokens, types, and families at each level in andLearning Maths 1B 2A
Learning Maths 1B Learning Maths 2A
Word list
(1,000)
Tokens (%) Types (%) Families Tokens (%) Types (%) Families
1 2661 (76.29) 303 (57.71) 231 (55.66) 1335 (84.02) 223 (76.63) 173 (75.22)
2 415 (11.90) 101 (19.24) 80 (19.28) 153 (9.63) 37 (12.71) 31 (13.48)
3 35 (1.00) 19 (3.62) 16 (3.86) 29 (1.83) 7 (2.41) 6 (2.61)
4 161 (4.62) 32 (6.10) 27 (6.51) 16 (1.01) 7 (2.41) 6 (2.61)
5 75 (2.15) 20 (3.81) 18 (4.38) 8 (0.50) 5 (1.72) 5 (2.17)
6 59 (1.69) 14 (2.67) 12 (2.89) 33 (2.08) 3 (1.03) 2 (0.87)
7 40 (1.15) 14 (2.67) 14 (3.37) 6 (0.38) 3 (1.03) 2 (0.87)
8 4 (0.11) 4 (0.76) 4 (0.96)
9 4 (0.11) 2 (0.38) 2 (0.48) 4 (0.25) 2 (0.69) 2 (0.87)
10 6 (0.17) 3 (0.57) 2 (0.48)
11 6 (0.17) 3 (0.57) 3 (0.72) 4 (0.25) 3 (1.03) 3 (1.30)
12 1 (0.03) 1 (0.19) 1 (0.24)
13 1 (0.03) 1 (0.19) 1 (0.24)
14 2 (0.06) 1 (0.19) 1 (0.24)
15
16
17 4 (0.11) 1(0.19) 1 (0.24)
18
19 4 (0.11) 2. (0.38) 2 (0.48)
20
Off-List 10 (0.29) 4. (0.76) ?? 1 (0.06) 1 (0.34) ??
Total 3488 (100) 525 (100) 415+? 1589 (100) 291 (100) 230+?
Table 4. Tokens, types, and families at each level in Math ViOlympic 4 and 5
Math ViOlympic 4 Math ViOlympic 5
Word list
(1,000)
Tokens (%) Types (%) Families Tokens (%) Types (%) Families
1 4689 (84.06) 424 (70.78) 303 (69.82) 4171 (81.13) 290 (67.29) 226 (65.89)
2 482 (8.64) 92 (15.36) 72 (16.59) 529 (10.29) 77 (17.87) 65 (18.95)
3 109 (1.95) 23 (3.84) 22 (5.07) 105 (2.04) 17 (3.94) 15 (4.37)
4 51 (0.91) 17 (2.84) 11 (2.53) 120 (2.33) 16 (3.71) 11 (3.21)
5 78 (1.40) 11 (1.84) 8 (1.84) 51 (0.99) 11 (2.55) 9 (2.62)
6 86 (1.54) 6 (1.00) 4 (0.92) 72 (1.40) 6 (1.39) 5 (1.46)
7
5 (0.09) 4 (0.67) 2 (0.46)
8
1 (0.02) 1 (0.23) 1 (0.29)
9 11 (0.20) 5 (0.83) 5 (1.15) 63 (1.23) 4 (0.93) 3 (0.87)
10 3 (0.05) 1 (0.17) 1 (0.23) 5 (0.10) 3 (0.70) 3 (0.87)
11 43 (0.77) 3 (0.50) 3 (0.69) 8 (0.16) 2 (0.46) 2 (0.58)
12
13
14
15 1 (0.02) 1 (0.17) 1 (0.23) 8 (0.16) 1(0.23) 1 (0.29)
16
7 (0.14) 2(0.46) 2 (0.58)
17
1 (0.02) 1 (0.17) 1 (0.23)
18
1 (0.02) 1 (0.17) 1 (0.23)
19
20
Off-List 18 (0.32) 10 (1.67) ?? 1 (0.02) 1 (0.23) ??
Total 5578 (100) 599 (100) 434+? 5141 (100) 431 (100) 343+?
T.N.M. Nhat / VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-10198
mathematical symbolism are repeatedly
present, the findings suggest that only a
small vocabulary is needed for YLs to
comprehend these mathematic problems.
The number of word-families a learner
would meet when s/he finished Math
ViOlympic 4, Math ViOlympic 5, Learning
Math 1B, Learning Math 2Aand is 434
+
,
343
+
, 415
+
, and 230
+
, respectively. The
data was shown to contain not only a
small number of word-families but also a
high frequency rate of encounter of each
word, which is strikingly similar across
the two series. A small number of these
word families are met from as high as 592
to six times (64.32%, 86.94%, 76.28%,
and 70.35%). The overall and unexpected
finding from a close analysis of the lists
of frequency indicates that these soaring
high percentages are typically represented
by function words and technical words.
By contrast, a substantial majority occur
merely once or twice in each book (Table
7). It should also be noticed that tokens
from this low-frequency group typically lie
with everyday common vocabulary for YLs’
world, namely family, school, animals, and
fruits.
Incidental learning theory indicates that if
unknown words are repeatedly encountered in
meaningful contexts, their meaning will
gradually be acquired (Nagy et al., 1985).
Research into L2 reading suggests that if
unknown words are encountered six or more
times, there is the potential for incidental
learning (Rott, 1999). Acquisition of word
meaning is also dependent on the contexts of
encounters (Webb, 2008). If words repeatedly
occur in highly informative contexts, their
meanings may be learned after a small number
of encounters. By contrast, in less informative
and/or misleading contexts, it could take as
many as 20 encounters for unknown words to
be learned (Webb, 2010). Therefore, it is
Table 5. Cumulative coverage (%) for each book
Word list Learning Maths 1B Learning Maths 2A Math ViOlympic 4 Math ViOlympic 5
1,000 76.29 84.02 84.06 81.13
2,000 88.19 93.65 92.70 91.42
3,000 89.19 95.48 94.65 93.46
4,000 93.81 96.49 95.56 95.76
5,000 95.96 96.99 96.96 96.78
6,000 97.65 99.07 98.50 98.18
7,000
98.80 99.45 98.59
8,000
98.91 98.20
9,000 99.02 99.70 98.79 99.43
10,000
99.19 98.84 99.53
11,000 99.36 99.95 99.61 99.69
12,000
99.39
13,000
99.42
14,000
99.48
15,000
99.63 99.85
16,000
99.99
17,000
99.59 99.65
18,000
99.67
19,000
99.70
20,000
Off-List 99.99 100.00 99.99 100.00
Tokens ≈100.00 ≈100.00 ≈100.00 ≈100.00
Table 6. K-1 sub-analysis in terms of content and function words for individual books
K1 Words Math ViOlympic 4 Math ViOlympic 5 Learning Maths 1B Learning Maths 2A
Function words 59.27% 52.69% 46.40% 50.16%
Content words 27.54% 31.24% 31.17% 34.36%
VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101 99
possible to deduce from the findings that the
chance for vocabulary growth in common
age-specific topics via doing ME is minimal.
A further analysis by means of
TextLexCompare yields the percentage of
recycled vocabulary in each set of data,
summarized in Table 8. The output shows
that the recycling index does not go above
85% for either set. This means that many or
most words throughout the two successive
books of each set are being met in density
environments of around 3 unknown words in
10, which doubles the density that learners can
handle. Research indicates that for learners
to be able to guess words in context and gain
adequate comprehension of written text it is
necessary to know at least 95% of the words
(Laufer, 1989). Moreover, comprehension
and incidental vocabulary learning through
reading are likely to increase if the percentage
of known words in a text is 98% (Nation,
2001). This result significantly supports the
finding that there may be very little incidental
vocabulary learning from doing ME for
primary school children.
5. Conclusions
The study is inspired by an appreciation
of the multisemiotic nature of MD. This is
essentially a new approach to mathematics
for teachers and students of mathematics,
offering penetrating insights into the functions
of the semiotic resources, individually and
integrally.
Overall, although all the three semiotic
resources are manipulated in all the books
analyzed, the distribution of each tends
to be unequal between the two series
analyzed. The visual component fails to be
paid due attention in the Math ViOlympic
series, which displays an overwhelming
predominance of the linguistic text. An
opposite extreme can be found in the
Learning Math series. As indispensable
as symbolism is in MD, this resource
is represented by a moderately high
percentage in all of the books analyzed.
Lexical profile analysis shows that
learners who finish both these books are
likely to encounter frequent words (at the
1000 level) enough to make significant gains
in vocabulary knowledge, with particular
reference to technical mathematic-specific
terms; however, Frequency analysis indicates
that around one half of the word-families will
not be met sufficiently for incidental learning
of vocabulary to occur. Text comparison
analysis further shows that the rate of new
word introduction in the higher-level book in
each set is more than most L2 learners will be
able to cope with.
Table 7. Number and percentage of encounters with word families (WF) in each book
Math ViOlympic 4 Math ViOlympic 5 Learning Math 1B Learning Math 2A
% No. of WF % No. of WF % No. of WF % No. of WF
6 times & > 64.32 86.94165 153 76.28 146 70.35 64
5-3 times 26.75 111 7.9 108 12.44 121 14.95 67
2-1 times 8.93 5.15370 214 11.28 299 14.7 167
Table 8. Recyclying index over each set
Math ViOlympic 4 & Math ViOlympic 5 Learning Maths 1B &Learning Maths 2A
Token 84.84% 74.94 %
Type 55.46% 49.47%
T.N.M. Nhat / VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101100
5.1. Pedagogical implications
The results of the close analysis from a
multisemiotic perspective have immediate
pedagogical implications as follows.
First, the test-orientated books published
by Vietnam Education Publishing House are
claimed “help students familiarize with the
fascinating test format, thinking stimulation
and computer practice before competition.
[…] to get the best competition score” (Đặng
Minh Tuấn & Nguyễn Thị Bích Phượng,
2016, p.3). The market-driven practices
have also resulted in these materials with a
predominance of the linguistic and symbolic
components. The findings therefore indicate
an urgent need for producing research-
informed graded materials beyond those
presently available in which we should not
lose sight of the multi-semiotic nature of MD.
Mathematical symbolism and visual images
have evolved to function in co-operation
with language. As the visual image plays
an increasingly important role in different
branches of mathematics (O’Halloran,
2004, p.148), with the impact of increased
computational ability, colorful computer-
generated visual images can now be generated
with minimal effort. Captivatingly presented,
these materials for primary-school children
may be of greatest importance to get learners
accustomed to MD in English as a foreign
language and to help them meet the initial
challenge in content-language integrated
learning that ME may at first present.
Second, for the Vietnamese YLs, although
incidental vocabulary learning may occur
through finishing the two books, the number
of words outside this specific domain which
can be acquired is likely to be limited. Thus,
teachers and learners should not consider
vocabulary learning as the primary goal
of doing ME. Learners may undoubtedly
benefit from other explicit ways to learn
vocabulary than through doing ME. To
facilitate understanding, it may be necessary
for teachers either to encourage guessing
from context or to provide glossaries so that
learners can check L1 translations quickly
when necessary.
5.2. Implications for further research
The data we have looked at in this article
suggest the following considerations for
further studies.
First, given the dearth of graded materials
in this area, there should be more research
to select and sequence resources, integrating
text-based with Internet-based texts, and to
provide smooth, principled access to them.
In addition to the obviously primary goal of
systematically targeting the field-specific
needs, efforts can be made to help facilitate
vocabulary growth opportunities that these
materials can offer. Frequency profiling
software can be used to modify and create
texts to pre-specified lexical profile and
coverage; and text comparison software can
be used to ensure degree of lexical recycling
over a series of chapters, books, and series.
Second, the results of the present
study suggest there may be potential for
incidental learning of the first 1,000 word-
families through engaging the YLs in doing
mathematics in English. However, while this
is a useful finding, further research to examine
experimentally through a controlled treatment
with the learners to provide a more accurate
assessment of the extent of transferring new
word learning to novel contexts is needed.
In addition, the sub-dimensions to the basic
learning condition, such as the spacing
between encounters should be taken into
consideration.
References
Đặng Minh Tuấn & Nguyễn Thị Bích Phượng (2016).
Math ViOlympic 5. Hanoi: Vietnam Education
Publishing House.
Đặng Minh Tuấn & Nguyễn Thị Hải (2016). Math
ViOlympic 4. Hanoi: Vietnam Education Publishing
House.
VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101 101
Cobb, T. (2010). Learning about language and learners
from computer programs. Reading in a Foreign
Language, 22(1), 181-200.
Krashen, S. (1989). We acquire vocabulary and spelling by
reading: Additional evidence for the input hypothesis.
The Modern Language Journal 73, , 440-464.
Krashen, S. (2003). Explorations in language acquisition
and use: The Taipei lectures. Portsmouth, NH:
Heinemann.
Laufer, B. & Sim, D. D. (1985). An attempt to
measure the threshold of competence for reading
comprehension. , (5), Foreign Language Annals 18
405-411.
Laufer, B. (1989). What percentage of text lexis is
essential for comprehension?. In C. Lauren & M.
Nordman (Eds.), Special Language: From Humans
Thinking to Thinking Machines, 316-323 Clevedon: .
Multilingual Matters.
Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. & Anderson, R. C. (1985).
Learning words from context. Reading Research
Quarterly, 20(2), 233-253.
Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning Vocabulary in Another
Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Nation, I.S.P. (2004). A study of the most frequent
word families in the British National Corpus. In P.
Bogaards & B. Laufer (Eds.), Vocabulary in a second
language: Selection, acquisition, and testing, 3–13.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Nation, I.S.P. (2006). How large a vocabulary is needed
for reading and listening? The Canadian Modern
Language Review, (1), 59-82.63
O’Halloran, K. L. (2004). Mathematical discourse
language, symbolism and visual images. London:
Continuum.
Rott, S. (1999). The effect of exposure frequency
on intermediate language learners’ incidental
vocabulary acquisition through reading. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition 21, (1), 589-619.
Tan, A. (2016a). . (Bilingual Learning Maths - 1B
version). Singapore Asia Publishers.
Tan, A. (2016b). . (Bilingual Learning Maths - 2A
version). Singapore Asia Publishers.
Webb, S. (2007). The effect of repetition on vocabulary
knowledge. Applied Linguistics, 28(1), 46-65.
Webb, S. (2008). The effects of context on incidental
vocabulary learning. Reading in a Foreign
Language, 20(2), 232-245.
Webb, S. (2010). A corpus driven study of the potential
for vocabulary learning through watching movies.
International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 15(4),
497-519.
PHÂN TÍCH ĐA THỨC DIỄN NGÔN TOÁN
BẰNG TIẾNG ANH DÀNH CHO LỨA TUỔI TIỂU HỌC
Tôn Nữ Mỹ Nhật
Khoa Ngoại ngữ, Trường Đại học Quy Nhơn,
170 An Dương Vương, Tp. Quy Nhơn, Bình Định, Việt Nam
Tóm tắt: Trẻ em Việt Nam ngày càng được tiếp cận nhiều thể loại nhằm phát triển năng lực
tiếng Anh, trong số đó các môn khoa học tự nhiên như môn Toán. Bài viết này khảo sát thể
loại diễn ngôn này từ góc nhìn đa tín hiệu. Cụ thể, công trình này nghiên cứu: (1) phân bố của
ba loại tín hiệu trong các tài liệu giải toán bằng tiếng Anh dành cho học sinh tiểu học; (2) số
lượng từ vựng yêu cầu đối với người học để giải các bài toán bằng tiếng Anh dành cho học sinh
tiểu học. Dữ liệu nghiên cứu là các sách luyện toán bằng tiếng Anh đang được sử dụng phổ biến
ở Việt Nam. Kết quả nghiên cứu cho thấy ý nghĩa giao tiếp của hai loại tín hiệu ký hiệu và hình
ảnh đối với thể loại diễn ngôn khoa học này, bên cạnh tín hiệu ngôn ngữ, các cấp độ từ vựng
tiếng Anh đối với người học để có thể hiểu được các bài toán đặt ra. Cuối cùng là một số thảo luận
về ý nghĩa thực tiễn đối với công việc biên soạn tài liệu, dạy và học toán bằng tiếng Anh đối với
lứa tuổi tiểu học.
Từ khóa: diễn ngôn toán, diễn ngôn đa thức, danh sách các từ thông dụng
| 1/9

Preview text:

A MULTIMODAL ANALYSIS OF MATHEMATICAL
DISCOURSE IN ENGLISH FOR YOUNG LEARNERS Ton Nu My Nhat*
Department of Foreign Languages, Quy Nhon University
170 An Duong Vuong, Quy Nhon, Binh Dinh, Vietnam Received 09 October 2017
Revised 02 November 2017; Accepted 27 November 2017
Abstract: Of multiple discourses where the Vietnamese young learners are increasingly engaged to
develop their English proficiency, English mathematical discourse (MD) has proved to be more and more
popular. This paper explores the materials in this realm from multisemiotic perspective. In particular, it
deals with two questions: (1) to what extent each of the three semiotic resources - language, visual images
and mathematical symbolism - is represented in the materials of learning mathematics in English (ME)
developed for young learners (YL) and (2) how many words the YLs need to know to comprehend the
language component of these materials. Data for illustrations and discussions are withdrawn from the
printed resources currently accessible in the Vietnamese context. The results offer insights into the functions
of other resources in constructing meanings apart from the well-established role of language as well as the
vocabulary load of these materials. The paper concludes with a discussion of pedagogical significance of
this study for material designers, teachers and learners and implications for further research.
Keywords: mathematical discourse, multisemiotic discourse, high frequency word list 1. Introduction
Specifically, the present study examines the
Mathematic Discourse (MD) is referred to as
following research questions: (1) To what
multisemiotic as it is constructed from more than
extent is each of the three semiotic resources
one semiotic resource - language, visual images
represented in the materials of learning
and mathematical symbolism (O’Halloran
mathematics in English (ME) developed for
2004, p.21). The view of mathematics as a
young learners (YLs)? and (2) How many
multisemiotic discourse is significant in a
words do YLs need to know to understand
pedagogical context as a better understanding
the vocabulary in ME and to what extent can
of the functions of mathematical symbolism and
these materials enhance incidental vocabulary
visual images permits a re-evaluation of the role
learning? Two major areas of interest are the
of language in the construction of meaning in
lexis specific to the field of Mathematics and
this naturalized domain. Such an understanding
that to children’s everyday world.
proves to be even more essential in the case of
content and language integrated learning (CLIL)
2. Mathematical discourse
in a foreign context, where the learners have to
O’Halloran’s (2004) study can be best
cope with both mathematic problems per se and
viewed as a first step towards a comprehensive a foreign language.
Systemic-functional Grammar for MD.
This study is an attempt to investigate MD
The major concern of this study is to
written in English for primary school learners.
investigate the multisemiotic nature of MD. * Tel.: 84-905242270
She developed theoretical frameworks for Email: tnmynhat70@gmail.com
mathematical symbolism and visual display. 94
T.N.M. Nhat / VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101
As reviewed in O’Halloran’s (2004, pp.
the sum of meanings derived from each of the
13-15) the multisemiotic approach, where
three resources. Intersemiosis refers to meaning
language, visual images and mathematical
which arises from the relations and shifts across
symbolism are considered semiotic resources,
the three semiotic resources; Intrasemiosis
originally stems from O’Toole’s (1994, 1995,
is meaning within one semiotic resource.
1999) extensions of Halliday’s (1978, 1994)
Royce (1998, p. 26, cited in O’Halloran, 2004:
Systemic-funtional approach to displayed art,
159) refers to intersemiosis as ‘intersemiotic
and Lemke’s (1998, 2000, 2003) early work
complementarity’ where ‘visual and verbal
in mathematical and scientific discourse.
modes semantically complement each other
Following are the central tenets which are
to produce a single textual phenomenon’. As
relevant to the present study.
Royce and Lemke (1998, cited in O’Halloran
(1) MD is considered as ‘multisemiotic’
2004, p. 159) explain, the product is ‘synergistic
construction; that is, discourses formed or ‘multiplicativ
e in that the result is greater
through choices from the functional sign than the sum of the parts.
systems of language, mathematical symbolism
Language, symbolism and visual images function together in and visual display.
mathematical discourse to create
(2) MD involves language, mathematical
a semantic circuit which permits
symbolism and visual images. The functions
semantic expansions beyond that
of each semiotic resource may be summarized
possible through the sum of the
three resources. Following this
as follows. Patterns of relations are encoded
view, the success of mathematics
and rearranged symbolically for the solution to
as a discourse stems from the fact
the problem. Due to the limited functionality
that it draws upon the meaning
of the symbolism, language functions as
potentials of language, visual images and the symbolism in
the meta-discourse to contextualize the
very specific ways. That is, the
problem, to explain the activity sequence
discourse, grammar and display
which is undertaken for the solution to the
systems for each resource have
mathematics problem. Visual images in
evolved to function as interlocking
the form of abstract and statistical graphs, system networks rather than
isolated phenomena. (O’Halloran
geometrical diagrams, and other types of 2004: 159)
diagrams and forms of visual display, mirror
(4) Mathematical printed texts are
our perceptual understanding of the world,
typically organized in very specific ways
showing the relations in a multi-dimensional
which simultaneously permit segration and
spatio-temporal format. They thus connect
integration of the three semiotic resources (p.
and extend common-sense experience to the
11). The systems of meaning for language,
mathematical symbolic descriptions.
symbolism and visual images are integrated
(3) MD depends on both intrasemiosis and
in such a way that the behaviour of physical
intersemiosis. As the types of meaning made
systems may be described. Choices from the
by each semiotic resource are fundamentally
three semiotic resources function integratively.
different (p.16), and thus the three semiotic
That is, the linguistic text and the graphs
resources fulfil individual functions, the success
contain symbolic elements and the symbolic
of mathematics depends on utilizing and
text contains linguistic elements. The symbolic
combining the unique meaning potentials of
elements may also be either spatially separated
language, symbolism and visual display in such
from the main body of the linguistic text or
a way that the semantic expansion is greater than
embedded within the linguistic text.
VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101 95 3. Methodology
vocabulary repetition across the books within 3.1. Materials each set and across the sets.
The books which served as the data of the
3.3. Procedures
present study comprise two sets. The first set
To achieve the aims, the texts were typed
consists of two books published by Vietnam
and computerized. The data was first closely
Education Publishing House - Math ViOlympic
analyzed in terms of the distribution of the
4 (Đặng Minh Tuấn & Nguyễn Thị Hải, 2016)
verbal, visual, and symbolic components.
and Math ViOlympic 5 (Đặng Minh Tuấn &
Whereas the statistics of the linguistic and
Nguyễn Thị Bích Phượng, 2016); the second
symbolic components were computationally
is two books published by Singapore Asia
performed, the images were manually
Publishers - Learning Maths 1B (Tan, A.
calculated. To analyze the vocabulary of the
2016a) and Learning Maths 2A (Tan, A.
books, the raw data were processed to omit the
2016b). Math ViOlympic 4 and Math
proper nouns. This is because many researchers
ViOlympic 5 are the only two published in
have taken the approach that proper nouns may
Vietnam so far in this realm. From the series
be easily understood by readers (e.g. Nation,
published by the foreign publisher, these two
2006); how proper nouns are handled makes a
books were chosen for analysis as these two
big difference to an output profile (Cobb, 2010).
are for the children of the same age groups as
The symbolic components and numbers, which
those in the first set. The number of problems
are inherent and pervasive of this genre, were
and of running words of the verbal texts in
also omitted. The data were then submitted to each book is shown in Table 1.
the vocabulary profile after being converted to
text files, using the BNC-20 wordlist.
Table 1. Number of problems and words in individual books analysed Book No. of Problems Running words Learning Maths 1B 381 3488 Learning Maths 2A 393 1589 Math ViOlympic 4 555 5578 Math ViOlympic 5 400 5141 Total 1,729 15,796 3.2. Instruments
4. Findings and discussion
The sets of materials were analysed
using Compleat Lexical Tutor developed by
4.1. Distribution of the three semiotic
Tom Cobb (available at http://www.lextutor). resources
VocabProfile gives all the information
As explicated above, the organisation of
regarding vocabularies of a text - the number
mathematical printed texts, typically involving
of type, token, word families, type-token ratio,
three semiotic resources, simultaneously
function and content words and even breaks any
permit segregation and integration of these
English text into its frequency levels according
componential elements. An in-depth analysis
to the thousand-levels scheme, Academic and
of the data, both computationally and manually,
off-list words, indicated by colours. Frequency
yielded insightful findings on the distribution
extracts frequency lists from the corpora.
of the resources, as shown in Table 2.
TextLexCompare is used to tract the amount of 96
T.N.M. Nhat / VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101
Table 2. Distribution of three semiotic resources Symbolic Images Language Total of elements Illustrative Integral Problems No. (%) No. (%) No. (%) No. (%) Learning Maths 1B 30 (7.87%) 20 (5.24%) 4 (1.04%) 351(92.12%) 381 Learning Maths 2A 158 (40.20%) 301(76.59%) 2 (0.50%) 31 (7.88%) 393 Math ViOlympic 4 555 (100%) 241 (43.42%) 26 (4.68%) 33 (5.94%) 555 Math ViOlympic 5 400 (100%) 214 (53.5%) 5 (1.25%) 37 (9.25%) 400
The most noticeable feature is the presence
be beneficial to the YLs’ vocabulary growth ,
of all the resources in all the books analysed.
the verbal data were submitted to VocabProfile
However, whereas the Learning Maths series
Frequency, and TextLexCompare. Table 3 and
tends to favor symbolic and imageries, the
4 summarize the data in terms of tokens, types, Math ViOlympic series displays an
and families of the two corpora, Learning
overwhelming predominance of language. All
Maths and Math ViOlympic, respectively; the
the problems in the Math ViOlympic series are
cumulative coverage for each book is shown
represented via language (100%); by contrast, in Table 5.
images account for less than 10 percent, of
Tables 3 and 4 show that the tokens
which approximately a half are just for the
spread over the 20 most frequent 1,000
illustrative purpose rather than functioning as
word families of the BNC. The importance
an integral component of the problems in
of knowing the most frequent word families
question. In other words, these images can be
is clearly demonstrated in the first rows of omitted without any inhibition to
these three tables. The first 1,000 word
understanding on the part of the learner.
families from the BNC account for up to
In the meantime, visuals are always
approximately four-fifths of tokens in the
contextualized in relation to the linguistic
problems in all these books – 76.29%,
text and/or the symbolic component in the
84.02%, 84.06%, and 81.13%. For example,
Learning Maths series. Another significant
regarding Math ViOlympic 4, the first row
finding from the data is the particularly high
indicates that 424 different word forms
proportion of images in Learning Math 1B,
(types) are the source of these 4689 tokens.
which is likely to result from an awareness
These 424 types reduce to 303 word-
of the meaningful function of this means in
families. Similarly, as for Learning Maths
MD in general and its motivating role to YLs
2A, the first 1,000 word families account for
of language in particular. Accordingly, in
1335 of the tokens, 225 of the types, and
this book, the two other resources make up a
173 of the families. It is useful to consider
mere 7.87% and 5.24%. Finally, the symbolic
the output in terms of word families because
component is moderately high in all the three
similarity in forms and meanings for tokens
other books (76.59%, 53.5%, and 43.42%).
from the same family may facilitate
This result is obviously due to the function of
understanding and retention. It is also clear
this semiotic resource in MD, as described in
that after the second 1,000 word-families, the third section.
the decreasing rate of the tokens tend to be
4.2. Features of the linguistic text
approximately the same across the four
To answer the second research question – to
books. From the third-1,000 onwards, the
what extent doing mathematics in English can
numbers of families thin out rapidly, which
VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101 97
suggests that the number of low frequency
first 1,000 word-families, the number of the words is few and far between.
function words tends to double that of the
Table 3. Tokens, types, and families at each level in Learning Maths 1B and 2A
Learning Maths 1B
Learning Maths 2A Word list Tokens (%) Types (%) Families Tokens (%) Types (%) Families (1,000) 1 2661 (76.29) 303 (57.71) 231 (55.66) 1335 (84.02) 223 (76.63) 173 (75.22) 2 415 (11.90) 101 (19.24) 80 (19.28) 153 (9.63) 37 (12.71) 31 (13.48) 3 35 (1.00) 19 (3.62) 16 (3.86) 29 (1.83) 7 (2.41) 6 (2.61) 4 161 (4.62) 32 (6.10) 27 (6.51) 16 (1.01) 7 (2.41) 6 (2.61) 5 75 (2.15) 20 (3.81) 18 (4.38) 8 (0.50) 5 (1.72) 5 (2.17) 6 59 (1.69) 14 (2.67) 12 (2.89) 33 (2.08) 3 (1.03) 2 (0.87) 7 40 (1.15) 14 (2.67) 14 (3.37) 6 (0.38) 3 (1.03) 2 (0.87) 8 4 (0.11) 4 (0.76) 4 (0.96) 9 4 (0.11) 2 (0.38) 2 (0.48) 4 (0.25) 2 (0.69) 2 (0.87) 10 6 (0.17) 3 (0.57) 2 (0.48) 11 6 (0.17) 3 (0.57) 3 (0.72) 4 (0.25) 3 (1.03) 3 (1.30) 12 1 (0.03) 1 (0.19) 1 (0.24) 13 1 (0.03) 1 (0.19) 1 (0.24) 14 2 (0.06) 1 (0.19) 1 (0.24) 15 16 17 4 (0.11) 1(0.19) 1 (0.24) 18 19 4 (0.11) 2. (0.38) 2 (0.48) 20 Off-List 10 (0.29) 4. (0.76) ?? 1 (0.06) 1 (0.34) ?? Total 3488 (100) 525 (100) 415+? 1589 (100) 291 (100) 230+?
Table 4. Tokens, types, and families at each level in Math ViOlympic 4 and 5
Math ViOlympic 4 Math ViOlympic 5 Word list Tokens (%) Types (%) Families Tokens (%) Types (%) Families (1,000) 1 4689 (84.06) 424 (70.78) 303 (69.82) 4171 (81.13) 290 (67.29) 226 (65.89) 2 482 (8.64) 92 (15.36) 72 (16.59) 529 (10.29) 77 (17.87) 65 (18.95) 3 109 (1.95) 23 (3.84) 22 (5.07) 105 (2.04) 17 (3.94) 15 (4.37) 4 51 (0.91) 17 (2.84) 11 (2.53) 120 (2.33) 16 (3.71) 11 (3.21) 5 78 (1.40) 11 (1.84) 8 (1.84) 51 (0.99) 11 (2.55) 9 (2.62) 6 86 (1.54) 6 (1.00) 4 (0.92) 72 (1.40) 6 (1.39) 5 (1.46) 7 5 (0.09) 4 (0.67) 2 (0.46) 8 1 (0.02) 1 (0.23) 1 (0.29) 9 11 (0.20) 5 (0.83) 5 (1.15) 63 (1.23) 4 (0.93) 3 (0.87) 10 3 (0.05) 1 (0.17) 1 (0.23) 5 (0.10) 3 (0.70) 3 (0.87) 11 43 (0.77) 3 (0.50) 3 (0.69) 8 (0.16) 2 (0.46) 2 (0.58) 12 13 14 15 1 (0.02) 1 (0.17) 1 (0.23) 8 (0.16) 1(0.23) 1 (0.29) 16 7 (0.14) 2(0.46) 2 (0.58) 17 1 (0.02) 1 (0.17) 1 (0.23) 18 1 (0.02) 1 (0.17) 1 (0.23) 19 20 Off-List 18 (0.32) 10 (1.67) ?? 1 (0.02) 1 (0.23) ?? Total 5578 (100) 599 (100) 434+? 5141 (100) 431 (100) 343+?
As shown in Table 6, it is also important
content words throughout the data.
to note that of these huge coverages of the
Assuming that proper nouns and 98
T.N.M. Nhat / VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101
Table 5. Cumulative coverage (%) for each book Word list
Learning Maths 1B
Learning Maths 2A
Math ViOlympic 4 Math ViOlympic 5 1,000 76.29 84.02 84.06 81.13 2,000 88.19 93.65 92.70 91.42 3,000 89.19 95.48 94.65 93.46 4,000 93.81 96.49 95.56 95.76 5,000 95.96 96.99 96.96 96.78 6,000 97.65 99.07 98.50 98.18 7,000 98.80 99.45 98.59 8,000 98.91 98.20 9,000 99.02 99.70 98.79 99.43 10,000 99.19 98.84 99.53 11,000 99.36 99.95 99.61 99.69 12,000 99.39 13,000 99.42 14,000 99.48 15,000 99.63 99.85 16,000 99.99 17,000 99.59 99.65 18,000 99.67 19,000 99.70 20,000 Off-List 99.99 100.00 99.99 100.00 Tokens ≈100.00 ≈100.00 ≈100.00 ≈100.00
Table 6. K-1 sub-analysis in terms of content and function words for individual books K1 Words
Math ViOlympic 4 Math ViOlympic 5 Learning Maths 1B
Learning Maths 2A Function words 59.27% 52.69% 46.40% 50.16% Content words 27.54% 31.24% 31.17% 34.36%
mathematical symbolism are repeatedly
merely once or twice in each book (Table
present, the findings suggest that only a
7). It should also be noticed that tokens
small vocabulary is needed for YLs to
from this low-frequency group typically lie
comprehend these mathematic problems.
with everyday common vocabulary for YLs’
The number of word-families a learner
world, namely family, school, animals, and
would meet when s/he finished Math fruits.
ViOlympic 4, Math ViOlympic 5, Learning
Incidental learning theory indicates that if
Math 1B, and Learning Math 2A is 434+,
unknown words are repeatedly encountered in
343+, 415+, and 230+, respectively. The
meaningful contexts, their meaning will
data was shown to contain not only a
gradually be acquired (Nagy et al., 1985).
small number of word-families but also a
Research into L2 reading suggests that if
high frequency rate of encounter of each
unknown words are encountered six or more
word, which is strikingly similar across
times, there is the potential for incidental
the two series. A small number of these
learning (Rott, 1999). Acquisition of word
word families are met from as high as 592
meaning is also dependent on the contexts of
to six times (64.32%, 86.94%, 76.28%,
encounters (Webb, 2008). If words repeatedly
and 70.35%). The overall and unexpected
occur in highly informative contexts, their
finding from a close analysis of the lists
meanings may be learned after a small number
of frequency indicates that these soaring
of encounters. By contrast, in less informative
high percentages are typically represented
and/or misleading contexts, it could take as
by function words and technical words.
many as 20 encounters for unknown words to
By contrast, a substantial majority occur
be learned (Webb, 2010). Therefore, it is
VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101 99
possible to deduce from the findings that the
offering penetrating insights into the functions
chance for vocabulary growth in common
of the semiotic resources, individually and
age-specific topics via doing ME is minimal. integrally.
Table 7. Number and percentage of encounters with word families (WF) in each book Math ViOlympic 4
Math ViOlympic 5 Learning Math 1B Learning Math 2A % No. of WF % No. of WF % No. of WF % No. of WF 6 times & > 64.32 165 86.94 153 76.28 146 70.35 64 5-3 times 26.75 111 7.9 108 12.44 121 14.95 67 2-1 times 8.93 370 5.15 214 11.28 299 14.7 167
Table 8. Recyclying index over each set
Math ViOlympic 4 & Math ViOlympic 5
Learning Maths 1B &Learning Maths 2A Token 84.84% 74.94 % Type 55.46% 49.47%
A further analysis by means of
Overall, although all the three semiotic
TextLexCompare yields the percentage of
resources are manipulated in all the books
recycled vocabulary in each set of data,
analyzed, the distribution of each tends
summarized in Table 8. The output shows
to be unequal between the two series
that the recycling index does not go above
analyzed. The visual component fails to be
85% for either set. This means that many or
paid due attention in the Math ViOlympic
most words throughout the two successive
series, which displays an overwhelming
books of each set are being met in density
predominance of the linguistic text. An
environments of around 3 unknown words in
opposite extreme can be found in the
10, which doubles the density that learners can
Learning Math series. As indispensable
handle. Research indicates that for learners
as symbolism is in MD, this resource
to be able to guess words in context and gain
is represented by a moderately high
adequate comprehension of written text it is
percentage in all of the books analyzed.
necessary to know at least 95% of the words
Lexical profile analysis shows that
(Laufer, 1989). Moreover, comprehension
learners who finish both these books are
and incidental vocabulary learning through
likely to encounter frequent words (at the
reading are likely to increase if the percentage
1000 level) enough to make significant gains
of known words in a text is 98% (Nation,
in vocabulary knowledge, with particular
2001). This result significantly supports the
reference to technical mathematic-specific
finding that there may be very little incidental
terms; however, Frequency analysis indicates
vocabulary learning from doing ME for
that around one half of the word-families will primary school children.
not be met sufficiently for incidental learning
of vocabulary to occur. Text comparison 5. Conclusions
analysis further shows that the rate of new
The study is inspired by an appreciation
word introduction in the higher-level book in
of the multisemiotic nature of MD. This is
each set is more than most L2 learners will be
essentially a new approach to mathematics able to cope with.
for teachers and students of mathematics, 100
T.N.M. Nhat / VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101
5.1. Pedagogical implications
facilitate understanding, it may be necessary
The results of the close analysis from a
for teachers either to encourage guessing
multisemiotic perspective have immediate
from context or to provide glossaries so that
pedagogical implications as follows.
learners can check L1 translations quickly
First, the test-orientated books published when necessary.
by Vietnam Education Publishing House are
5.2. Implications for further research
claimed “help students familiarize with the
The data we have looked at in this article
fascinating test format, thinking stimulation
suggest the following considerations for
and computer practice before competition. further studies.
[…] to get the best competition score” (Đặng
First, given the dearth of graded materials
Minh Tuấn & Nguyễn Thị Bích Phượng,
in this area, there should be more research
2016, p.3). The market-driven practices
to select and sequence resources, integrating
have also resulted in these materials with a
text-based with Internet-based texts, and to
predominance of the linguistic and symbolic
provide smooth, principled access to them.
components. The findings therefore indicate
In addition to the obviously primary goal of
an urgent need for producing research-
systematically targeting the field-specific
informed graded materials beyond those
needs, efforts can be made to help facilitate
presently available in which we should not
vocabulary growth opportunities that these
lose sight of the multi-semiotic nature of MD.
materials can offer. Frequency profiling
Mathematical symbolism and visual images
software can be used to modify and create
have evolved to function in co-operation
texts to pre-specified lexical profile and
with language. As “the visual image plays
coverage; and text comparison software can
an increasingly important role in different
be used to ensure degree of lexical recycling
branches of mathematics” (O’Halloran,
over a series of chapters, books, and series.
2004, p.148), with the impact of increased
Second, the results of the present
computational ability, colorful computer-
study suggest there may be potential for
generated visual images can now be generated
incidental learning of the first 1,000 word-
with minimal effort. Captivatingly presented,
families through engaging the YLs in doing
these materials for primary-school children
mathematics in English. However, while this
may be of greatest importance to get learners
is a useful finding, further research to examine
accustomed to MD in English as a foreign
experimentally through a controlled treatment
language and to help them meet the initial
with the learners to provide a more accurate
challenge in content-language integrated
assessment of the extent of transferring new
word learning to novel contexts is needed.
learning that ME may at first present.
In addition, the sub-dimensions to the basic
Second, for the Vietnamese YLs, although
learning condition, such as the spacing
incidental vocabulary learning may occur
between encounters should be taken into
through finishing the two books, the number consideration.
of words outside this specific domain which
can be acquired is likely to be limited. Thus, References
teachers and learners should not consider
Đặng Minh Tuấn & Nguyễn Thị Bích Phượng (2016).
vocabulary learning as the primary goal
Math ViOlympic 5. Hanoi: Vietnam Education
of doing ME. Learners may undoubtedly Publishing House.
Đặng Minh Tuấn & Nguyễn Thị Hải (2016). Math
benefit from other explicit ways to learn
ViOlympic 4. Hanoi: Vietnam Education Publishing
vocabulary than through doing ME. To House.
VNU Journal of Foreign Studies, Vol.33, No.6 (2017) 93-101 101
Cobb, T. (2010). Learning about language and learners Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
from computer programs. Reading in a Foreign
Nation, I.S.P. (2006). How large a vocabulary is needed
Language, 22(1), 181-200.
for reading and listening? The Canadian Modern
Krashen, S. (1989). We acquire vocabulary and spelling by
Language Review, (1), 59-82. 63
reading: Additional evidence for the input hypothesis.
O’Halloran, K. L. (2004). Mathematical discourse –
The Modern Language Journal, 73, 440-464.
language, symbolism and visual images. London:
Krashen, S. (2003). Explorations in language acquisition Continuum.
and use: The Taipei lectures. Portsmouth, NH:
Rott, S. (1999). The effect of exposure frequency Heinemann.
on intermediate language learners’ incidental
Laufer, B. & Sim, D. D. (1985). An attempt to
vocabulary acquisition through reading. Studies in
measure the threshold of competence for reading
Second Language Acquisition 21 , (1), 589-619.
comprehension. Foreign Language Annals, 18 (5),
Tan, A. (2016a). Learning Maths - 1B. (Bilingual 405-411.
version). Singapore Asia Publishers.
Laufer, B. (1989). What percentage of text lexis is
Tan, A. (2016b). Learning Maths - 2A. (Bilingual
essential for comprehension?. In C. Lauren & M.
version). Singapore Asia Publishers.
Nordman (Eds.), Special Language: From Humans
Webb, S. (2007). The effect of repetition on vocabulary
Thinking to Thinking Machines, 316-323. Clevedon:
knowledge. Applied Linguistics, 28(1), 46-65. Multilingual Matters.
Webb, S. (2008). The effects of context on incidental
Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. & Anderson, R. C. (1985).
vocabulary learning. Reading in a Foreign
Learning words from context. Reading Research
Language, 20(2), 232-245.
Quarterly, 20(2), 233-253.
Webb, S. (2010). A corpus driven study of the potential
Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning Vocabulary in Another
for vocabulary learning through watching movies.
Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 15(4),
Nation, I.S.P. (2004). A study of the most frequent 497-519.
word families in the British National Corpus. In P.
Bogaards & B. Laufer (Eds.), Vocabulary in a second
language: Selection, acquisition, and testing
, 3–13.
PHÂN TÍCH ĐA THỨC DIỄN NGÔN TOÁN
BẰNG TIẾNG ANH DÀNH CHO LỨA TUỔI TIỂU HỌC Tôn Nữ Mỹ Nhật
Khoa Ngoại ngữ, Trường Đại học Quy Nhơn,
170 An Dương Vương, Tp. Quy Nhơn, Bình Định, Việt Nam
Tóm tắt: Trẻ em Việt Nam ngày càng được tiếp cận nhiều thể loại nhằm phát triển năng lực
tiếng Anh, trong số đó có các môn khoa học tự nhiên như môn Toán. Bài viết này khảo sát thể
loại diễn ngôn này từ góc nhìn đa tín hiệu. Cụ thể, công trình này nghiên cứu: (1) phân bố của
ba loại tín hiệu trong các tài liệu giải toán bằng tiếng Anh dành cho học sinh tiểu học; và (2) số
lượng từ vựng yêu cầu đối với người học để giải các bài toán bằng tiếng Anh dành cho học sinh
tiểu học. Dữ liệu nghiên cứu là các sách luyện toán bằng tiếng Anh đang được sử dụng phổ biến
ở Việt Nam. Kết quả nghiên cứu cho thấy ý nghĩa giao tiếp của hai loại tín hiệu ký hiệu và hình
ảnh đối với thể loại diễn ngôn khoa học này, bên cạnh tín hiệu ngôn ngữ, và các cấp độ từ vựng
tiếng Anh đối với người học để có thể hiểu được các bài toán đặt ra. Cuối cùng là một số thảo luận
về ý nghĩa thực tiễn đối với công việc biên soạn tài liệu, dạy và học toán bằng tiếng Anh đối với lứa tuổi tiểu học.
Từ khóa: diễn ngôn toán, diễn ngôn đa thức, danh sách các từ thông dụng