Behavioral Biases on Investment Decision A Case Study in Indonesia - Copy - Tài liệu tham khảo | Đại học Hoa Sen

Behavioral Biases on Investment Decision A Case Study in Indonesia - Copy - Tài liệu tham khảo | Đại học Hoa Sen và thông tin bổ ích giúp sinh viên tham khảo, ôn luyện và phục vụ nhu cầu học tập của mình cụ thể là có định hướng, ôn tập, nắm vững kiến thức môn học và làm bài tốt trong những bài kiểm tra, bài tiểu luận, bài tập kết thúc học phần, từ đó học tập tốt và có kết quả

Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240 12311231
Print ISSN: 2288-4637 / Online ISSN 2288-4645
doi:10.13106/jafeb.2021.vol8.no3.1231
Behavioral Biases on Investment Decision:
A Case Study in Indonesia
Kartini KARTINI
1
, Katiya NAHDA
2
Received: November 30, 2020 Revised: February 07, 2021 Accepted: February 16, 2021
Abstract
A shift in perspective from standard finance to behavioral finance has taken place in the past two decades that explains how cognition and
emotions are associated with financial decision making. This study aims to investigate the influence of various psychological factors on
investment decision-making. The psychological factors that are investigated are differentiated into two aspects, cognitive and emotional
aspects. From the cognitive aspect, we examine the influence of anchoring, representativeness, loss aversion, overconfidence, and optimism
biases on investor decisions Meanwhile, from the emotional aspect, the influence of herding behavior on investment decisions is analyzed. .
A quantitative approach is used based on a survey method and a snowball sampling that result in 165 questionnaires from individual
investors in Yogyakarta. Further, we use the One-Sample -test in testing all hypotheses. The research findings show that all of the variables, t
anchoring bias, representativeness bias, loss aversion bias, overconfidence bias, optimism bias, and herding behavior have a significant
effect on investment decisions. This result emphasizes the influence of behavioral factors on investor’s decisions. It contributes to the
existing literature in understanding the dynamics of investor’s behaviors and enhance the ability of investors in making more informed
decision by reducing all potential biases.
Keywords: Behavioral Finance, Investment Decisions, Emotional Bias, Cognitive Bias
JEL Classification Code: G41, G11, D91, D81, F65
Not only modern portfolio theory but also a range of other
conventional finance theories, such as capital asset pricing
model (CAPM) (Treynor, 1961; Sharpe, 1964; Lintner,
1965; Mossin, 1969) and efficient market hypothesis (EMH)
(Fama, 1970) use the same assumptions, that investors are
always rational.
Barberis and Thaler (2003) explained that rational
behavior should cover two things. First, when investors
receive new information, they will update their beliefs
appropriately and accurately. Second, based on the new
beliefs, the investors will make the right decisions consistent
with the explanation of conventional finance theories.
Hence, biases will not occur in an investment decision,
as each individual is considered to have the capability of
selecting the best alternatives among various options that
are available, based on complete calculations, theories,
concepts, and the right approaches.
A basic question put forward by a few theorists of
behavioral finance, is “are investors always rational?”
(Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; De Bondt & Thaler, 1985;
Shefrin & Statman, 1985; Shiller, 1987). According to them,
the assumption of investor rationality is not easily fulfilled,
1
First Author and Corresponding Author. Department of Management,
Faculty of Business and Economics, Universitas Islam Indonesia,
Yogyakarta, Indonesia [Postal Address: Kampus Terpadu Universitas
Islam Indonesia, Jl. Kaliurang KM. 14, 5 Sleman, Yogyakarta 55584,
Indonesia] Email: 903110103@uii.ac.id ; kartiniafif1@gmail.com
2
Department of Management, Faculty of Business and
Economics, Universitas Islam Indonesia, Yogyakarta, Indonesia.
Email: katiya.nahda@uii.ac.id
© Copyright: The Author(s)
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution
Non-Commercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits
unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the
original work is properly cited.
1. Introduction
Optimal portfolio investment as explained by Markowitz
(1952) is focused on two things, (1) how to maximize
investment returns at a given level of risk, or (2) minimizing
risks at a certain level of return. In building a portfolio
theory, Markowitz (1952) assumed that all investors are
rational individuals in making a decision. Therefore, all the
decisions made are expected to generate the highest utility
possible through a variety of rational analysis processes.
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–12401232
since investors make decisions when presented with
alternatives that involve risk, probability, and uncertainty.
People choose between different options (or prospects) and
how they estimate (many times in a biased or incorrect way)
the perceived likelihood of each of these options. They also
state that financial decision-makers not only involve logical
and rational considerations, but also psychological aspects
that are at times irrational, involving intuition. Hence, it may
deviate from the rationality assumption.
Behavioral Economics is the study of psychology as
it relates to the economic decision-making processes of
individuals and institutions. Behavioral economics reveals
systematic deviations from rationality exposed by investors.
Individuals are victims of their cognitive biases that lead
to the existence of financial market inefficiencies and
anomalies (Al-Mansour, 2020). Literally, decision making is
a process of selecting the best alternative from a number of
possible options available in complex situations (Hirschey
& Nofsinger, 2008). The complexity of it then makes the
investor simplify decision-making in drawing the right
conclusions (Shefrin, 2007). Therefore, the information
acquired and the level of investor ability to process
the information is highly affected by the quality of the
decisions made. Further, Shefrin (2007) stated that a range
of behavioral bias practices or irrational behavior is mainly
caused by investor’s limited ability to analyze information
and the emotional factor in decision making.
The concept of behavioral finance has arisen from the
assumption that human beings as social as well as intellectual
creatures involve mind and emotion in decision making.
According to Hirschey and Nofsinger (2008), behavioral
finance is defined as “A study of cognitive errors and
emotions in financial decisions”. They explained that the
concept of behavioral finance is a study of financial decision-
making caused by emotional and cognitive factors. Pompian
(2006) divided decision-making biases into two categories
cognitive and emotional biases. The former is a bias that
is associated with the thought process, while the latter is
associated with feelings and emotions. Asri (2013) classified
cognitive bias into 3 groups as suggested by Shefrin (2000):
a. The bias of simplifying decision-making processes
by using rules of thumb is known as heuristic bias.
Heuristics are commonly defined as cognitive
shortcuts or rules of thumb that simplify decisions,
especially under conditions of uncertainty. This
group comprises availability, hindsight, and
representativeness biases.
b. The bias of reaction to information based on the
information’s frameworks is called a framing effect.
The framing effect is a cognitive bias where people
decide on options based on whether the options are
presented with positive or negative connotations;
as a loss or as a gain. Framing bias occurs when people
make a decision based on the way the information is
presented, as opposed to just on the facts themselves.
The same facts presented in two different ways
can lead to people making different judgments or
decisions. Framing is as important as a substance
that was previously ignored by traditional finance.
This group consists of overreaction, conservatism,
anchoring, and confirmation biases.
c. The bias of understanding information and self-
adjusting to the market price is known as prior bias.
The prior bias, heuristic bias, and framing effect, will
at last cause prices to deviate from their fundamental
value, thus the market will be inefficient. This group
includes optimism, overconfidence, and mental
accounting biases.
Sha and Ismail (2020) explained that investors make
decisions based on the available information, and the issue is
related to how they build their perception of that information.
In this context, the investors should be aware of the different
types of cognitive biases that may lead them to a much
better or worst position. They found that the investors are
influenced by different cognitive biases and it depends on
the gender of the investor.
Based on previous research findings and phenomena, this
research makes a further investigation about the influence of
biases, both cognitive and emotional, on investment decision
making. Shefrin (2002) described that behavioral finance is
not a science to defeat the market. The most important part
of this concept is the recognition of the existing risk from an
investor sentiment or the risk that arises due to psychological
factors that are sometimes larger than the fundamental risk.
This study was corroborated by Kartini and Nuris
(2015) who stated that the various biases that occur can
be detrimental, as it can lead to a risk miscalculation that
may occur. Besides, such biases are also difficult to control
because they are invisible and directly linked to thought
processes involving emotions or feelings. Despite the
controlling difficulty, Olsen (1998) warned that the main
purpose of behavioral finance is understanding the influence
of psychological factors systematically in the financial
market so that each individual will be more prudent in
decision making.
This research attempts to investigate the influence of
various psychological factors on investment decision-making.
The psychological factors to be investigated are differentiated
into two aspects as explained by Pompian (2006), cognitive
and emotional aspects. Accordingly, this study examines the
influence of anchoring, representativeness, loss aversion,
overconfidence, and optimism biases on investor decisions.
Meanwhile, the latter aspect to be examined is the influence
of herding behavior on investment decisions.
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240 1233
2. Literature Review
2.1. Investment Decision
Investment decisions are the process of choosing
investment from various alternatives that are commonly
affected by the past investment’s returns and the expected
returns in the future (Subash, 2012). There are two kinds of
investors in making investment decisions, rational investor
and irrational investor. Rational investors are those who
make a decision merely based on logical thinking and
information about the investment prospect. While irrational
investors decide based on their psychological aspect which
creates biases in investment decisions.
2.2. Prospect Theory
Prospect theory is proposed by Kahneman and Tversky
(1979). In general, it explains how investors make decisions
under certain risks. According to them, individuals assess
their loss and gain perspectives asymmetrically. Thus,
contrary to the expected utility theory (which models the
decision that perfectly rational agents would make), prospect
theory aims to describe the actual behavior of people. They
found that losses hurt about twice as much as gains make
us feel good. That is people feel the pain of loss twice as
strongly as they feel pleasure at an equal gain. The thought
that the pain of losing is psychologically about twice as
powerful as the pleasure of gaining is known as loss aversion.
The other implication of prospect theory is people tend to
take larger risks to avoid losses, rather than take risks to earn
profits. To put it another way, investors will be inclined to
be risk-averse, when coming across profits and switch to be
risk-takers when perceiving losses. This finding contrasts
with the expected utility theory from Markowitz (1952)
who stated that a rational investor will exhibit consistent
behavior, whether he/she is a risk-averse or a risk-taker
under any circumstances.
2.3. Heuristic Theory
The term heuristic was introduced by Tversky and
Kahneman (1974) who described that the decisions made
amid complexities and conditions of uncertainty are mostly
based on the beliefs concerning the likelihood of uncertain
events. Uncertainty in events is uncertainty regarding
either the occurrence of an event. These beliefs then form
a heuristic way of thinking, by which people tend to use
rules of thumb to simplify the decision-making processes.
This view was strengthened by De Bondt et al. (2008) that
individuals (investors) have a bias in their belief that will
affect how they think and make decisions. Fromlet (2001)
defined heuristics as “the use of experience and practical
efforts” that is an effort to interpret information quickly by
relying on experiences accompanied by intuition. It explains
how individuals or groups make decisions under conditions
of uncertainty. Investors frequently make mistakes in
decision making because they use rules of thumb as a basis
in processing the information. On the one hand, a heuristic
approach can facilitate faster decision making. This approach
may result in biases or errors that occur systematically.
Tversky and Kahneman (1974) classified heuristic bias into
3 types - representativeness, availability, and anchoring
biases that will be investigated in this study.
2.4. Framing Theory
The subsequent discussion of cognitive bias after
heuristics dealing is framing According to Frensidy (2016), .
traditional finance assumes that framing is transparent.
Meanwhile, behaviorists think of it differently, many frames
are not so transparent that investors have difficulty seeing
it clearly. Consequently, the decisions made will be highly
dependent on how the information is framed or presented.
Based on the previous experiment, Frensidy (2016) described
someone (suppose called Budi), in a different way by using
the same information on two separate groups, group A and
B. In group A, Budi is said to be a smart, diligent, impulsive,
critical, stubborn, and jealous person, whereas, in group B,
Budi is described as a jealous, stubborn, critical, impulsive,
diligent, and smart person. The same characteristics about
Budi but presented in reverse order turn out to significantly
influence the groups’ assessment results. The experiment
results reveal that the characteristics mentioned earlier
have more influence than those mentioned later. Group A
significantly asses Budi better than group B do. He argued
that there are two reasons which explain such phenomena.
First, one’s concentration level may decrease with the
increasing amount of information to be absorbed, so that
the information placed behind gets less attention. Second,
first impressions usually receive more weight than the
information that comes after. These two things then lead to
anchoring bias to occur.
3. Hypotheses Development
3.1. Anchoring Bias and Investment Decisions
According to Tversky and Kahneman (1974), anchoring
bias occurs when people rely too much on pre-existing
information or the first information they find when
making decisions (anchor). Then, adjustment is made on
such perception. Investors who are affected by this bias
tend to underlie their investment decisions on one certain
information, regardless of whether the information is first
acquired, or it is the only information available which made
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–12401234
people highly rely on it. Ackert and Deaves (2009) defined
overconfidence as the tendency of a person to overestimate
knowledge, ability, and the accuracy of the information
that an investor possesses, or the tendency to become too
optimistic about the future and ability. Such an investor must
be wary of anchoring bias. Despite the different information
available, people tend to be inclined to the first-owned
information in making a decision.
Many investors in the capital market experience
anchoring bias that most of them continue to remember the
buying price of shares in their portfolio. The selling decision
is frequently based on the buying price as the reference
point. Investors decide to sell their shares sooner when the
price is above the reference point. Besides buying price, the
highest price of shares that has ever been achieved during a
certain period also frequently become reference price. Yet,
many investors are not willing to cut loss cause they refer to
such reference prices (Frensidy, 2016). Thus, a hypothesis is
proposed as follows:
H1: Anchoring bias will affect investment decisions.
3.2. Representativeness Bias and
Investment Decisions
Representativeness bias is someone’s tendency to make
decisions based on certain stereotypes or prior knowledge
or experiences. Representativeness bias happens when
people make decisions only by limited observations to
acquire information from the surrounding environment
and ignore other information (Baker & Nofsinger, 2002;
Ritter, 2003; Shefrin, 2000). Representativeness bias tends
to lead investors to overreact during processing information
in making decisions (Kahneman & Riepe, 1998). It is
supported by the findings of Franses (2007) and Marsden
et al. (2008) who revealed that the representativeness bias
can cause over-reaction behavior to occur as reflected in the
stock prices. On the other hand, investors who are affected
by this bias can also ignore or not pay close attention to
important events that may happen in the future. Hence, they
do not protect themselves from such unexpected events
(Yoong, 2010).
Representativeness bias can lead someone to make a
wrong conclusion. Higher-priced products are often decided
with higher quality than lower-priced ones, although there
is a likelihood that prices do not always reflect quality.
Chen et al. (2007) explained that the common stereotypes
in the capital market are investors tend to interpret the
good characteristics of a company, such as product quality,
reliable managers, and high growth as the characteristics of
the company that has a worthwhile investment.
The other error of representativeness bias that often
occurs in the financial market as described by Frensidy
(2016) is the assumption that past performance is the best
indicator to predict future performance. In other words,
investors often believe that past rates of return represent
future expected return. If a company announces successive
profit increases, investors will assume that it will continue to
rise and consider this company a good company, which means
good investment. Therefore, investors expect higher returns
for past winners’ stocks and use this trend as a stereotype for
future stock movements (Lakonishok et al., 1994; Ackert &
Deaves, 2010). Thus, based on the explanations and reviews
on earlier studies, a hypothesis is proposed as follows:
H2: Representativeness bias will affect investment
decisions.
3.3. Loss Aversion and Investment Decisions
According to Pompian (2006), loss aversion is the
tendency to prefer avoiding losses to acquiring equivalent
gains. Loss aversion is a tendency where investors are so
fearful of losses that they focus on trying to avoid a loss
more so than on making gains. The more one experiences
losses, the more likely they are to become prone to loss
aversion. Research on loss aversion shows that investors
feel the pain of a loss more than twice as strongly as they
feel the enjoyment of making a profit. The concept of loss
aversion has emerged as an implication of prospect theory
that investors are not risk-averse, but loss averse. Such a
thing occurs because the psychological impacts of losses are
greater than those of profits. To put it another way, investors
tend to feel more stressed by potential losses in comparison
to potential gains with an equivalent value. Therefore, they
will be more prudent in investment to reduce the risk of
losses (Barberis & Thaler, 2003).
H3: Loss aversion bias will affect investment decisions.
3.4. Overconfidence Bias and
Investment Decisions
Another behavioral bias that is also often found among
investors is overconfidence. Overconfidence bias means
that the individual is outrightly confident of his decisions
and he overestimates or exaggerates his ability to perform a
task. Decision-makers incline to overestimate the knowledge
and information that they possessed, also ignore the public
information available. The investors with overconfidence
bias override models and data because they convince
themselves that they know better. They may not always know
better, and by ignoring the early signs of potential damage,
they cause themselves more harm than good. (Lichtenstein
& Fischhoff, 1977). Baker and Nofsinger (2002) defined
overconfidence as a form of excessive self-confidence that
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240 1235
the information owned can be utilized properly because of
having good analytical skills. In fact, this is only an illusion
of belief, due to lack of experience and having a shortcoming
in interpreting available information.
In short, overconfidence is a condition in which investors
believe and consider their abilities are above the average of
other investors and have an unrealistic level of self-evaluation
(Odean, 1999; Pompian, 2006). Frischhoff et al. (1977)
asserted that in an uncertain world of investment, investors
are inclined to make overconfident decisions. Those who are
overconfident usually think that they know more than they
do, so they tend to believe that they are better or smarter than
others (Shiller, 2000). When the number of market participants
who are overconfident is large, then the aggregate reactions
that occur in the market will be far from being ration8al.
According to Frensidy (2016), an individual’s inclination
to be overconfident may be caused by two things. First,
except for those who are depressed, everyone positively
judges themselves. Second, psychologically, people want
to control the situation and their surroundings and believe
that they can do that. Further, he revealed that there are four
financial implications of this bias. First, investors can take the
wrong position in buying and selling shares because they fail
to realize that they do not have the advantage of information
or analysis. Second, investors are inclined to trade more
frequently which results in higher transaction costs. Third,
overconfident investors are inclined to set prediction
intervals that are too narrow. Finally, overconfident investors
will be surprised more often than expected.
A handful of studies have been conducted to investigate
the influence of overconfidence bias on the financial market.
Overconfidence behavior unconsciously influences investors
to do excessive trading (Benos, 1998; Daniel et al., 1998;
Graham et al., 2005; Odean, 1999; Pompian, 2006; Toma,
2015; Ullah et al., 2017). Besides affecting the frequency
of transactions, overconfidence bias also affects trading
volume. The higher the overconfident level, the greater the
volume traded (Statman et al., 2003). These results indicate a
positive influence of overconfidence bias towards investment
decision making. Besides, Bakar and Yi (2016) also revealed
that the level of overconfidence that occurs also depends on
individual gender.
Those research findings are in line with Pompian (2006)
who found that the belief of overconfidence in skills can
cause mistakes in decision making that make investors
trade excessively. The overconfident investor also tends to
overestimate investment returns and underestimate risks. If
the actual return is lower than the expected return, they will
associate it with an unfortunate condition (Miller, 1975).
Overall, these various factors will have a positive impact on
investment decisions.
H4: Overconfidence bias will affect investment decisions.
3.5. Optimism Bias and Investment Decisions
Optimism bias often relates to overconfidence bias.
Nofsinger (2005) explained that both overconfidence and
optimism biases are caused by the same psychological
factors an illusion of knowledge and illusion of control.
The former is a condition in which an individual feel highly
confident with the information they possessed. It affects his/
her belief in the chance level of success that may be achieved.
Such belief then leads to perceived control over the results
to be gained (the illusion of control). The illusion of control
is the tendency for people to overestimate their ability to
control events; for example, it occurs when someone feels a
sense of control over outcomes that they demonstrably do not
influence. Thus, control illusions are defined as a situation in
which people frequently believe that they have influenced
the results obtained from an uncontrolled event. When an
illusion of knowledge and illusion of control evolve further,
there will appear as excessive optimism (Shefrin, 2007).
Shefrin (2007) defined optimism bias as one who inclines
to overestimate success (the likelihood of obtaining results
as desired) and underestimating the risk of failure. Further,
optimism bias is an investor’s expectation or belief that their
portfolio performance will always generate a positive return
(Hoffmann et al., 2013). The important thing from this bias is that
there is a likelihood that investors make investment decisions
excessively. The higher level of optimism bias that occurred,
the higher investor’s expectation of their portfolio performance.
This positive expectation then spurs them to increase the
frequency and volume of trading, even though there is a high
probability that the actual may deviate from the expectations
(Pompian, 2006). Khan et al., (2017) and Ullah et al. (2017)
found a relationship between past portfolio yields and the level
of investor optimism that had an impact on investment decisions.
H5: Optimism bias will affect investment decisions.
3.6. Herding Behavior and Investment Decisions
Banerjee (1992) and Hirshleifer and Teoh (2003) explained
herding behavior as a people behavior that tends to follow the
actions of other people rather than following their owned-
beliefs or owned-information in the making decision. This
behavior is considered irrational behavior as investors decide
based on other’s decisions in the market (Altman, 2012).
Herding behavior is often found among investors in emerging
markets and mostly occurred during market stress situations
(Rahayu et al., 2020). According to Humra (2014), herding
behavior occurs when a group of investors make investment
decisions based on collective information from a group of
investors and ignore other information. As a result, when
the group majority makes a wrong decision, it will turn to
significant market price deviations.
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–12401236
The finding of Chang et al. (2000) showed that herding
practices are more prevalent in developing countries, which
then is supported by Chiang and Zheng (2010) and Zheng
et al. (2017) concerning the herding practices in Asian stock
exchanges (China, South Korea, Singapore, Malaysia, and
Indonesia). Herd mentality bias refers to investors’ tendency
to follow and copy what other investors are doing. They are
largely influenced by emotion and instinct, rather than by
their own independent analysis. In Indonesia, the research
findings related to herding behavior are still contradictive,
even though they are tested by the same methods. Sari
(2012), and Purba and Faradynawati (2012) revealed there
had been herding practices in Indonesia, whereas Narasanto
(2012) did not find herding practices. Furthermore, Bowe
and Domuta (2004), using the Lakonishok et al. (1992)
method, found that herding behavior in the Indonesian Stock
Exchange was mostly dominated by foreign investors.
H6: Herding behavior will affect investment decisions.
4. Methodology
The population of this study is investors over the age
of 17 years and based in Yogyakarta and based on random
sampling that results in 165 respondents. To determine the
sample size, we use the Slovin formula. It provides the
sample size ( ) using the known population size ( ) and n N
the acceptable error value ( ). Slovin’s formula gives the e
researcher an idea of how large the sample size needs to be
to ensure a reasonable accuracy of results.
n
Z
2
2
4 Moe
Z = Level of confidence, this study uses a 95%
confidence level
Moe The maximum tolerable error rate is 8% =
n = sample size
We collect the data using questionnaires based on the
5-Likert scale, with 1 Strongly Disagree; 2 Disagree; = =
3 = Neutral; 4 Agree; and 5 Strongly Agree. We define = =
only score 4 and 5 that considered investor decisions
are influenced by behavioral bias, otherwise, it does not
(Altamimi, 2006). The total number of questions from all
variables are 42 questions 5 questions for anchoring bias,
8 questions for representativeness bias, 5 questions for loss
aversion, 7 questions for overconfidence bias, 7 questions
for optimism bias, and 4 questions for herding behavior.
Six independent variables are used in the model -anchoring
bias, representativeness bias, loss aversion, overconfidence
bias, optimism bias, and herding behavior, while the
dependent variable is investment decisions. The data is first
analyzed based on validity and reliability test to validate the
questionnaires using Bivariate Pearson correlation (Pearson
Product Moment).
r
xy
N XY X Y
N X X N
Y Y
( )( )
[{ ( ) }{
( )
}]
2 2
2 2
r
xy = Pearson Correlation Coefficient
X = Scores for each question or statement item
Y = Scores for total question items or statements
X = total scores in X distribution
Y = total scores in Y distribution
X
2
= total squares of each X score
Y
2
= total squares of each Y score
N = total subjects
The reliability of items that test the degree of stability,
consistency, predictive power, and accuracy are measured
based on the Cronbach alpha formula:
α
=
K
K
S S
S
r
x
1
2
1
2
2
α = Cronbach’ alpha reliability coefficient
K total question items tested =
s
1
2
total item score variants =
S
X
2
= Variance of test scores (all items)
Then, One sample -test is used to test the effect of anchoring t
bias, representativeness bias, loss aversion, overconfidence
bias, optimism bias, and herding behavior on investor decisions.
The formula for one-sample -test is used as follows:t
Z
X
S n
x
5. Results and Discussion
Table 1 displays the information about the characteristics
of the respondents. The results of the Pearson Bivariate
correlation test on the total questionnaire items indicate that
all of them are valid based on the -value of each item which r
is greater than the value of the -table. The Cronbach’s alpha r
score also shows similar results where the value for each item
is greater than 0.6. The score for each variable - anchoring
bias, representativeness bias, loss aversion, over-evidence
bias, optimism bias, and herding behavior are 0.61, 0.77,
0.67, 0.86, 0.78, and 0.75 respectively. The Kolmogorov-
Smirnov test is used to check the normality and it shows that
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240 1237
the data is distributed normally. The results of one sample
t-test indicate that the average value of each variable is
greater than 3.00 and all of the hypotheses are significant at
1% alpha. It indicates that most of the investors in Yogyakarta
tend to be affected by anchoring bias, representativeness
bias, loss aversion, overconfidence bias, optimism bias, and
herding behavior in making investment decisions.
5.1. Anchoring Bias and Investment Decisions
The result of the first hypothesis suggests that there is an
inclination of the investors to sell the stocks based on buying
price as the reference price. The investors make quick decisions
to sell their shares when the selling price exceeds the buying
price. Besides buying price, the highest price that was achieved
during a certain period also becomes the reference price.
Besides, investors decide to buy stocks based on the past stocks’
performance which means the investors overestimate their
own opinions and expertise. This finding is in line with that of
Frensidy (2016), Vijaya (2014), Rekik and Boujelbene (2013),
Luong and Doan (2011), and Masomi and Ghayekhloo (2010).
5.2. Representativeness Bias and
Investment Decisions
The result of the second hypothesis testing indicates that
representativeness bias has a significant influence on invest-
ment decisions. Investors tend to make decisions simply based
on limited information from the surroundings and ignore other
information or the important events that may happen in the
future. Hence, they are less prepared for unexpected events
or information. They also consider that past performance as
the best indicator to predict future performance. The repre-
sentativeness bias further supports the notion that people fail
to properly calculate and utilize probability in their decisions.
The research finding is consistent with Toma (2015), Vijaya
(2016), and Virigineni and Bhaskara (2017).
5.3. Loss Aversion and Investment Decisions
The third hypothesis result shows that investors are
prudent in deciding to buy or sell the shares to avoid the loss.
They focus more on avoiding the losses instead of gaining
higher profits. Very often, stocks are bought without much
research. So, once the stock price goes up, investors fear that
it may go down as fast as it went up. Such thinking makes
them sell the stocks too soon. Then come instances where
the stock price has gone down after an investor has bought
it. This tends to happen when the primary reason for buying
the stock was a recent upsurge. Hence, when the value of
their portfolio investment decreases, they prefer to retain it
as they hope that it would increase to the previous price in
the future. On the other hand, they tend to sell their stock so
early when the investment value increases. These findings
support the concept of disposition effect and are in line with
that of Luong and Doan (2011), Ngoc (2014), Khan (2017),
Kimeu et al. (2016), and Rekik and Boujelbene (2013).
5.4. Overconfidence Bias and
Investment Decisions
We also find in this study that overconfidence bias has a
significant influence on investment decisions. Considering
that the major respondents are college students, there is a
high level of probability that they tend to have a higher level
of enthusiasm and motivation to get into the investment
world. However, enthusiasm and motivation themselves are
not enough to be a good investor. They need to develop more
investment skills and broaden the knowledge of investment
which they do not have enough. Stock investment is a long-
term investment that has the highest risk compared with
other types of investments, such as mutual funds or bonds.
This is worth noting for young investors who are very
vulnerable to overconfidence bias. This empirical finding is
consistent with Toma (2015), Bakar and Yi (2016), and Ullah
et al. (2017).
Table 1: Sample Descriptive
Demographic Factors
Number of
Respondents
Percentage
Gender Male 94 57%
Female 71 43%
Level of
Education
Postgraduate 5 3%
Bachelor’s
degree
82 49.7%
Associate
degree
5 3%
Senior high
school
73 44.2%
Investment
selected
Stocks 141 85.5%
Bonds 4 2.4%
Mutual funds 15 9.1%
Others 5 3%
Table 2: The Result of One-Sample T-Test
Variables Mean Significance Result
Anchoring bias H0 rejected3.84 0.000
Representativeness bias H0 rejected3.78 0.000
Loss aversion H0 rejected4.09 0.000
Overconfidence bias H0 rejected3.27 0.000
Optimism bias 3.95 0.000 H0 rejected
Herding behavior 3.30 0.000 H0 rejected
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–12401238
5.5. Optimism Bias and Investment Decisions
The result of the fifth hypothesis also describes that
optimism bias affects investment decisions significantly.
The optimism bias is an expectation or belief that the
future performance will always better than the past return
(Hoffmann et al., 2013). As the respondents are dominated
by the young investors, who are highly susceptible to
be overconfident, it will be followed by a high level of
optimism. Overconfidence and optimism biases are caused
by the illusion of knowledge and illusion of control. The
illusion of knowledge is a condition where a person feels
very confident about the information he/she has so that it
has an impact on his/her belief in their chance of success.
It supports by the empirical finding of Khan et al. (2017),
Fatima and Waqas (2016), and Ullah et al. (2017).
5.6. Herding Behavior and Investment Decisions
This study also found that herding behavior has a
significant influence on investment decisions. It indicates
that investors tend to rely on collective information from
other investors rather than personal information. In this
respect, the investors react impulsively to the changes
found in others’ decisions as they prefer others’ investment
choices to their own choices. They put little attention on the
company’s prospects and believe more about what others
decide in the market. This finding is in line with the studies
of Vijaya (2014), Ngoc (2014), Ranjbar et al. (2014), and
Kumar and Goyal (2015).
6. Conclusion
Our research findings suggest that anchoring bias,
representativeness bias, loss aversion, overconfidence bias,
optimism bias, and herding behavior affect significantly the
investor’s decisions. There are a few opportunities for much
more comprehensive research on investors’ behavioral biases.
References
Ackert, L. F., & Deaves R. (2010). Behavioral finance psychology,
decision making, and markets. Cincinnati, OH: South-Western,
Cengage Learning.
Al-Mansour, B. Y. (2020). Cryptocurrency market: Behavioral
finance perspective. Journal of Asian Finance, Economics, and
Business, 7(12), 159–165. https://doi.org/10.13106/jafeb.2020.
vol7.no12.159
Altamimi, H. A. H. (2006). Factors influencing individual investor
behavior: An empirical study of the UAE financial market. The
Business Review. 5(2), 225–232. https://www.researchgate.net/
profile/Hussein_Al-Tamimi/publication/257936341_Factors_
Influencing_Individual_Investor_Behaviour_An_Empirical_
study_of _the_ UAE_Financia l_Mar ketsThe_Busi ness_
Review_Cambridge_Vol5No_2225-232_2006/links/00b495
3ab9c4e8ba85000000.pdf
Altman, M. (2012). Behavioral economics for dummies.
Mississauga: John Wiley & Sons.
Asri, M. (2013). Behavioral finance. Yogyakarta: BPFE-
Yogyakarta.
Bakar, S., & Yi, A. C. (2016). The impact of psychological factors
on investors’ decision-making in the Malaysian stock market:
a case of Klang Valley and Pahang. Procedia Economics
and Finance, 35, 319–328. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2212-
5671(16)00040-X
Baker, H. K., & Nofsinger J. R. (2002). Psychological bias of
investors. Financial Services Review. 11(2), 97–116. https://
scinapse.io/papers/172315172
Banerjee, A. V. (1992). A simple model of herd behavior. The
Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(3), 797–817. https://doi.
org/10.2307/2118364
Barberis, N., & Thaler, R. (2003). A survey of behavioral finance.
Handbook of the Economics of Finance, 25(2), 1053–1128.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1574-0102(03)01027-6
Benos, A. V. (1998). Aggressiveness and survival of overconfident
traders. , Journal of Financial Markets 1(3–4), 353-383. https://
doi.org/10.1016/S1386-4181(97)00010-4
Bowe, M., & Domuta, D. (2004). Investor herding during the
financial crisis: A clinical study of the Jakarta Stock Exchange.
Pacific-Basin Finance Journal, 12(4), 387–418. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.pacfin.2003.09.003
Chang, E. C., Cheng, J. W., & Khorana, A. (2000). An examination
of herd behavior in equity markets. International Perspective
Journal of Banking and Finance, 24, 1651–1679. https://doi.
org/10.1016/S0378-4266(99)00096-5
Chen, G., Kim, K. A., & Nofsinger, J. R. (2007). Trading
performance, disposition effect, overconfidence, represent-
ativeness bias, and experience of emerging market investors.
Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 20, (4), 425–451.
https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.957504
Chiang, T. C., & Zheng, D. (2010). An empirical analysis of
herd behavior in global stock markets. Journal of Banking
and Finance, 34, 1911–1921. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jbankfin.2009.12.014
Daniel, K., Hirshleifer, D., & Subrahmanyam, A. (1998). Investor
psychology and security market under and overreaction. Journal
of Finance, 53(6), 1839–1885. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-
1082.00077
De Bondt, W. F. M., & Thaler, R. (1985). Does the stock market
overreact? , Journal of Finance 40(3), 793–805. https://doi.
org/10.2307/2327804
De Bondt, W., Muradoglu, G., Shefrin, S., & Staikouras, S. K.
(2008). Behavioral finance: Quo Vadis? Journal of Applied
Finance 18, (2), 1–15. https://ssrn.com/abstract=269814
Fama, E. F. (1970). Efficient capital markets: A review on theory
and empirical work. (2), 383–417. Journal of Finance, 25
https://doi.org/10.2307/2325486
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240 1239
Fatima, N., & Waqas, M. (2016). Impact of optimistic bias and
availability bias on investment decision making with the
moderating role of financial literacy. First International
Conference of Indigenous Resource Management challenges
and Opportunities at University of Management Sciences and
Information Technology Kotli, Azaad Kashmir 1415 April
2016 (pp. 1–28).
Franses, P. H. (2007). Experts adjusting model-based forecast and
the law of small numbers (Report No. EI 2007-42). Econometric
Institute, Erasmus University Rotterdam. https://repub.eur.nl/
pub/10563
Frensidy, B. (2016). Agile and tactical in the capital market:
Armed with behavioral finance. Jakarta: Salemba Empat.
Frischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1977). Knowing
with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance 3, (4), 552–564. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-
1523.3.4.552
Fromlet, H. (2001). Behavioral finance-theory and practical
application. Business Economics, 36(3), 63–69. https://doi.
org/10.2139/ssrn.2329573
Graham, J. R., Harvey, C. R., & Huang, H. (2005). Investor
competence, trading frequency, and home bias (NBER
Working Paper 11426). National Bureau of Economic
Research https://www.nber.org/system/files/working_papers/
w11426/w11426.pdf
Hirschey, M., & Nofsinger R. J. (2008). Investment: Analysis and
behavioral. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill/Irwin.
Hirshleifer, D., & Teoh, S. H. (2003). Herd behavior and cascading
in capital markets: A review and synthesis. European Financial
Management 9, (1), 25–66.
Hoffmann, A. O., Post, T., & Pennings M. E. (2013). Individual
investor perception and behavior during the financial crisis.
Journal of Banking and Finance, 37(1), 60–74. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jbankfin.2012.08.007
Humra, Y. (2014). Behavioral finance: An introduction to the
principles governing investor behavior in stock markets.
International Journal of Financial Management, 5(2), 23–30.
http://iaset.us/download/archives/2-35-1456481625-3.%20
Abstract.pdf
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis
of decision under risk. (2), 263–291. https://Econometrica, 47
doi.org/10.2307/1914185
Kahneman, D., & Riepe, M. W. (1998). Aspects of investor
psychology: Beliefs, preferences, and biases investment
advisors should know about. Journal of Portfolio Management,
24(4), 52–65. https://doi.org/10.3905/jpm.1998.409643
Kartini, K., & Nuris, F. N. (2015). The influence of illusions of
control, overconfidence, and emotional biases on investment
decisions. Jurnal Inovasi dan Kewirausahaan, 4(2), 115–123.
https://doi.org/10.20885/ajie.vol4.iss2.art6
Khan, M. U. (2017). Impact of availability bias and loss aversion
bias on investment decision making, moderating role of risk
perception. Journal of Modern Developments in General
Management & Administration, 1(1), 17–28. http://www.
impactjournals.us/download/archives/1-82-1502108083-3.
Khan, M. T., Chong, L. L., & Tan, S. H. (2017). Perception of past
portfolio returns, optimism, and financial decisions. Review of
Behavioral Finance, 9(1), 79–98. https://doi.org/10.1108/RBF-
02-2016-0005
Kimeu, C. N., Anyago. W., & Rotich, G. (2016). Behavioural
factors influencing investment decisions among individual
investors in Nairobi Securities Exchange. The Strategic Journal
of Business & Change Management, 3(4), 1243–1258. http://
www.strategicjournals.com/index.php/journal/article/view/377
Kumar, S., & Goyal, N. (2015). Behavioral biases in investment
decision making; A systematic literature review. Qualitative
Research in Financial Markets, 7(1), 88–108.
Lakonishok, J., Shleifer, A., & Vishny, R. W. (1992). The impact
of institutional trading on, stock price. Journal of Financial
Economics, 32(1), 23–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-
405X(92)90023-Q
Lakonishok, J., Shleifer, A., & Vishny, R. W. (1994). Contrarian
investment, extrapolation, and risk. Journal of Finance, (5), 49
1541–1578. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1994.tb04772
Lichtenstein, S., & Fischhoff, B. (1977). Do those who know more
also know more about how much they know? Organizational
Behavior and Human Performance. 20(2), 159–83. https://doi.
org/10.1177/002224379202900304
Lintner, J. (1965). Security prices, risk, and maximum gains from
diversification. Journal of Finance, 20(4), 587–615. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1965.tb02930
Luong, L. P., & Doan, T. T. H. (2011). Behavioral individual
investors’ decision making and performance a survey at the
Ho Chi Minh stock exchange [Master Thesis, Umeå School of
Business]. http://umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:423263/
FULLTEXT02.pdf
Markowitz, H. (1952). Portfolio selection. Journal of Finance,
7(1), 77–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1952.tb01525
Marsden, A., Veeraraghavan, M., & Ye. M. (2008). Heuristics of
representativeness, anchoring, and adjustment, and leniency:
impact on earnings forecasts by Australian analysts. Quarterly
Journal of Finance and Accounting, 47(2), 83–102. http://hdl.
handle.net/2292/8518
Masomi, S. R., & Ghayekhloo, S. (2010). Consequences of human
behaviors’ in economic: The effects of behavioral factors
in investment decision making at Teheran Stock Exchange.
International Conference on Business and Economics Research,
1, 1–4. http://ipedr.com/vol1/50-B10068.pdf
Miller, D. T. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of
causality: Fact or fiction. Psychological Bulletin, (2), 82
213–225. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076486
Mossin, J. (1969). Security pricing and investment criteria in
competitive markets. American Economic Review, (5), 59
749–756. https://www.jstor.org/stable/1810673
Narasanto, I. T. (2012). Herding behavior an experience
in Indonesian stock market (No, 5589) [Master Thesis,
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–12401240
Perpustakaan Universitas Gadjah Mada, Yogyakarata] http://
etd.repository.ugm.ac.id/home/detail_pencarian/55899
Ngoc, L. T. B. (2014). The behavior pattern of individual investors
in the stock market. International Journal of Business and
Management 9, (1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.5539/ijbm.v9n1p1
Nofsinger, J. R. (2005). Social mood and financial economics.
Journal of Behavioral Finance, 6(3), 144–160. https://doi.
org/10.1207/s15427579jpfm0603_4
Odean, T. (1999). Do investors trade too much? American Economic
Review 89, , 1279–98. https://doi.org/10.2139/SSRN.94143
Olsen. R. A. (1998). Behavioral finance and its implication for
stock price volatility. (2), 10–18. Financial Analyst Journal, 52
https://doi.org/10.2469/faj.v54.n2.2161
Pompian, M. M. (2006). Behavioral finance and wealth
management. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Purba, A. V., & Faradynawati, A. A. (2012). An examination of herd
behavior in the Indonesian Stock Market. Indonesian Capital
Market Review, 4(1), 1–10. https://doi/org/10.21002/icmr.v4i1.985
Rahayu, S., Rohman, A., & Harto, P. (2020). Herding behavior
model in investment decision on Emerging Markets:
Experimental in Indonesia. Journal of Asian Finance,
Economics, and Business, 8(1), 53–59. https://doi.
org/10.13106/jafeb.2021.vol8.no1.053
Ranjbar, M. H., Abedini, B., & Jamali, M. (2014). Analyzing the
effective behavioral factors on the investors’ performance
in Tehran Stock Exchange (TSE). International Journal of
Technical Research and Applications, 2(8), 2320–8163.
Rekik, Y. M., & Boujelbene, Y. (2013). Determinants of individual
investors’ behaviors: Evidence from Tunisian Stock Market.
IOSR Journal of Business and Management, 8(2), 109–119.
https://doi.org/10.9790/487X-082109119
Ritter, J. R. (2003). Behavioral finance. Pacific-Basin Finance
Journal, 11(4), 429–437. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0927-
538X(03)00048-9
Sari, A. M. (2012). Herding behavior in Indonesia and the impact of
the global stock market. Yogyakarta: FEB UGMMaster Thesis.
Sha, N., & Ismail, M. Y. (2020). Behavioral investor types and
financial market players in Oman. Journal of Asian Finance,
Economics, and Business, 8(1), 285–294. https://doi.
org/10.13106/jafeb.2021.vol8.no1.285
Sharpe, W. F. (1964). Capital asset prices: A theory of market
equilibrium under the condition of risk. Journal of Finance, 19(3),
425–442. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1964.tb02865
Shefrin, H., & Statman, M. (1985). The disposition to sell winners
too early and riding losers too long. Journal of Finance, (3), 40
777–790. https://doi.org/10.2307/2327802
Shefrin, H. (2000). Beyond greed and fear: Understanding
behavioral finance and the psychology of investing. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
Shefrin H. (2002). Behavioral decision making, forecasting,
game theory, and role-play. International Journal of
Forecasting 18, (3), 375–382. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-
2070(02)00021-3
Shefrin, H. (2007). Behavioral corporate finance: Decision that
creates value. McGraw-Hill/Irwin
Shiller, R. J. (1987). Investor behavior in the October 1987
stock market crash: Survey evidence (Working Paper
2446). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.
org/10.3386/w2446
Shiller, R. J. (2000). Princeton, NJ: Irrational exuberance.
Princeton University Press.
Statman, M., Thorley, S., & Vorkink, K. (2003). Investor
overconfidence and trading volume. Review of Financial
Studies, 19(4), 1531–1565. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.168472
Subash, R. (2012). Role of behavioral finance in portfolio
investment decision: Evidence from India [Master Thesis,
Charles University in Prague]. https://is.cuni.cz/webapps/zzp/
detail/110165/?lang=en
Toma, F. M. (2015). Behavioral biases of the investment decisions
of Romanian investors on the Bucharest Stock Exchange.
Procedia Economics and Finance, 32, 200–207. https://doi.
org/10.1016/S2212-5671(15)01383-0
Treynor. J. L. (1961). [Unpublished Market value, time, and risk
manuscript]. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2600356
Tversky, A., & Kahneman D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty:
Heuristics and biases. Science, New Series, 185(4157),
1124–1131.
Ullah, I., Ullah. A., & Rehman, N. U. (2017). Impact of
overconfidence and optimism on investment decision.
International Journal of Information, Business, and
Management 9, (2), 231–243. http://ejournal.ukrida.ac.id/ojs/
index.php/Akun/article/download/1802/1821/
Vijaya, E. (2014). An empirical analysis of influential factors on
investment behavior of retail investors’ in the Indian Stock
Market: A behavioral perspective. International Journal in
Management and Social Science, 2(12), 296–308. https://
www.indianjournals.com/ijor.aspx?target=ijor:ijmss&volume=
2&issue=12&article=027
Vijaya, E. (2016). An empirical analysis on the behavioral
pattern of Indian retail equity investors. Journal of Resources
Development and Management, 16, 103–112. https://core.
ac.uk/download/pdf/234696209.pdf
Virigineni, M., & Bhaskara, R. (2017). Contemporary developments
in behavioral finance. International Journal of Economics and
Financial, 7(1), 448–459. https://www.econjournals.com/
index.php/ijefi/article/view/3809
Yoong, J. (2010). Making financial education more effective:
lessons from behavioral economics (Working paper Session II).
Behavioral Economics and Financial Education.
Zheng, D., Li. H., & Chiang, T. C. (2017). Herding with
industries: Evidence from Asian stock markets. International
Review of Economics and Finance, 51, 487–509. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.iref.2017.07.005
| 1/10

Preview text:

Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240 1231
Print ISSN: 2288-4637 / Online ISSN 2288-4645
doi:10.13106/jafeb.2021.vol8.no3.1231
Behavioral Biases on Investment Decision:
A Case Study in Indonesia
Kartini KARTINI1, Katiya NAHDA2
Received: November 30, 2020 Revised: February 07, 2021 Accepted: February 16, 2021 Abstract
A shift in perspective from standard finance to behavioral finance has taken place in the past two decades that explains how cognition and
emotions are associated with financial decision making. This study aims to investigate the influence of various psychological factors on
investment decision-making. The psychological factors that are investigated are differentiated into two aspects, cognitive and emotional
aspects. From the cognitive aspect, we examine the influence of anchoring, representativeness, loss aversion, overconfidence, and optimism
biases on investor decisions Meanwhile, .
from the emotional aspect, the influence of herding behavior on investment decisions is analyzed.
A quantitative approach is used based on a survey method and a snowball sampling that result in 165 questionnaires from individual
investors in Yogyakarta. Further, we use the One-Sample t-test in testing all hypotheses. The research findings show that all of the variables,
anchoring bias, representativeness bias, loss aversion bias, overconfidence bias, optimism bias, and herding behavior have a significant
effect on investment decisions. This result emphasizes the influence of behavioral factors on investor’s decisions. It contributes to the
existing literature in understanding the dynamics of investor’s behaviors and enhance the ability of investors in making more informed
decision by reducing all potential biases.
Keywords: Behavioral Finance, Investment Decisions, Emotional Bias, Cognitive Bias
JEL Classification Code: G41, G11, D91, D81, F65 1. Introduction
Not only modern portfolio theory but also a range of other
conventional finance theories, such as capital asset pricing
Optimal portfolio investment as explained by Markowitz
model (CAPM) (Treynor, 1961; Sharpe, 1964; Lintner,
(1952) is focused on two things, (1) how to maximize
1965; Mossin, 1969) and efficient market hypothesis (EMH)
investment returns at a given level of risk, or (2) minimizing
(Fama, 1970) use the same assumptions, that investors are
risks at a certain level of return. In building a portfolio always rational.
theory, Markowitz (1952) assumed that all investors are
Barberis and Thaler (2003) explained that rational
rational individuals in making a decision. Therefore, all the
behavior should cover two things. First, when investors
decisions made are expected to generate the highest utility
receive new information, they will update their beliefs
possible through a variety of rational analysis processes.
appropriately and accurately. Second, based on the new
beliefs, the investors will make the right decisions consistent
with the explanation of conventional finance theories.
1 First Author and Corresponding Author. Department of Management,
Hence, biases will not occur in an investment decision,
Faculty of Business and Economics, Universitas Islam Indonesia,
as each individual is considered to have the capability of
Yogyakarta, Indonesia [Postal Address: Kampus Terpadu Universitas
Islam Indonesia, Jl. Kaliurang KM. 14, 5 Sleman, Yogyakarta 55584,
selecting the best alternatives among various options that
Indonesia] Email: 903110103@uii.ac.id ; kartiniafif1@gmail.com
are available, based on complete calculations, theories, 2 Department of Management, Faculty of Business and
concepts, and the right approaches.
Economics, Universitas Islam Indonesia, Yogyakarta, Indonesia.
A basic question put forward by a few theorists of Email: katiya.nahda@uii.ac.id
behavioral finance, is “are investors always rational?” © Copyright: The Author(s)
(Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; De Bondt & Thaler, 1985;
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution
Non-Commercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits
Shefrin & Statman, 1985; Shiller, 1987). According to them,
unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the
original work is properly cited.
the assumption of investor rationality is not easily fulfilled, 1232
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240
since investors make decisions when presented with
as a loss or as a gain. Framing bias occurs when people
alternatives that involve risk, probability, and uncertainty.
make a decision based on the way the information is
People choose between different options (or prospects) and
presented, as opposed to just on the facts themselves.
how they estimate (many times in a biased or incorrect way)
The same facts presented in two different ways
the perceived likelihood of each of these options. They also
can lead to people making different judgments or
state that financial decision-makers not only involve logical
decisions. Framing is as important as a substance
and rational considerations, but also psychological aspects
that was previously ignored by traditional finance.
that are at times irrational, involving intuition. Hence, it may
This group consists of overreaction, conservatism,
deviate from the rationality assumption.
anchoring, and confirmation biases.
Behavioral Economics is the study of psychology as
c. The bias of understanding information and self-
it relates to the economic decision-making processes of
adjusting to the market price is known as prior bias.
individuals and institutions. Behavioral economics reveals
The prior bias, heuristic bias, and framing effect, will
systematic deviations from rationality exposed by investors.
at last cause prices to deviate from their fundamental
Individuals are victims of their cognitive biases that lead
value, thus the market will be inefficient. This group
to the existence of financial market inefficiencies and
includes optimism, overconfidence, and mental
anomalies (Al-Mansour, 2020). Literally, decision making is accounting biases.
a process of selecting the best alternative from a number of
possible options available in complex situations (Hirschey
Sha and Ismail (2020) explained that investors make
& Nofsinger, 2008). The complexity of it then makes the
decisions based on the available information, and the issue is
investor simplify decision-making in drawing the right
related to how they build their perception of that information.
conclusions (Shefrin, 2007). Therefore, the information
In this context, the investors should be aware of the different
acquired and the level of investor ability to process
types of cognitive biases that may lead them to a much
the information is highly affected by the quality of the
better or worst position. They found that the investors are
decisions made. Further, Shefrin (2007) stated that a range
influenced by different cognitive biases and it depends on
of behavioral bias practices or irrational behavior is mainly the gender of the investor.
caused by investor’s limited ability to analyze information
Based on previous research findings and phenomena, this
and the emotional factor in decision making.
research makes a further investigation about the influence of
The concept of behavioral finance has arisen from the
biases, both cognitive and emotional, on investment decision
assumption that human beings as social as well as intellectual
making. Shefrin (2002) described that behavioral finance is
creatures involve mind and emotion in decision making.
not a science to defeat the market. The most important part
According to Hirschey and Nofsinger (2008), behavioral
of this concept is the recognition of the existing risk from an
finance is defined as “A study of cognitive errors and
investor sentiment or the risk that arises due to psychological
emotions in financial decisions”. They explained that the
factors that are sometimes larger than the fundamental risk.
concept of behavioral finance is a study of financial decision-
This study was corroborated by Kartini and Nuris
making caused by emotional and cognitive factors. Pompian
(2015) who stated that the various biases that occur can
(2006) divided decision-making biases into two categories
be detrimental, as it can lead to a risk miscalculation that
– cognitive and emotional biases. The former is a bias that
may occur. Besides, such biases are also difficult to control
is associated with the thought process, while the latter is
because they are invisible and directly linked to thought
associated with feelings and emotions. Asri (2013) classified
processes involving emotions or feelings. Despite the
cognitive bias into 3 groups as suggested by Shefrin (2000):
controlling difficulty, Olsen (1998) warned that the main
purpose of behavioral finance is understanding the influence
a. The bias of simplifying decision-making processes
of psychological factors systematically in the financial
by using rules of thumb is known as heuristic bias.
market so that each individual will be more prudent in
Heuristics are commonly defined as cognitive decision making.
shortcuts or rules of thumb that simplify decisions,
This research attempts to investigate the influence of
especially under conditions of uncertainty. This
various psychological factors on investment decision-making.
group comprises availability, hindsight, and
The psychological factors to be investigated are differentiated representativeness biases.
into two aspects as explained by Pompian (2006), cognitive
b. The bias of reaction to information based on the
and emotional aspects. Accordingly, this study examines the
information’s frameworks is called a framing effect.
influence of anchoring, representativeness, loss aversion,
The framing effect is a cognitive bias where people
overconfidence, and optimism biases on investor decisions.
decide on options based on whether the options are
Meanwhile, the latter aspect to be examined is the influence
presented with positive or negative connotations;
of herding behavior on investment decisions.
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240 1233 2. Literature Review
efforts” that is an effort to interpret information quickly by
relying on experiences accompanied by intuition. It explains
2.1. Investment Decision
how individuals or groups make decisions under conditions
of uncertainty. Investors frequently make mistakes in
Investment decisions are the process of choosing
decision making because they use rules of thumb as a basis
investment from various alternatives that are commonly
in processing the information. On the one hand, a heuristic
affected by the past investment’s returns and the expected
approach can facilitate faster decision making. This approach
returns in the future (Subash, 2012). There are two kinds of
may result in biases or errors that occur systematically.
investors in making investment decisions, rational investor
Tversky and Kahneman (1974) classified heuristic bias into
and irrational investor. Rational investors are those who
3 types - representativeness, availability, and anchoring
make a decision merely based on logical thinking and
biases that will be investigated in this study.
information about the investment prospect. While irrational
investors decide based on their psychological aspect which 2.4. Framing Theory
creates biases in investment decisions.
The subsequent discussion of cognitive bias after 2.2. Prospect Theory
heuristics dealing is framing According . to Frensidy (2016),
traditional finance assumes that framing is transparent.
Prospect theory is proposed by Kahneman and Tversky
Meanwhile, behaviorists think of it differently, many frames
(1979). In general, it explains how investors make decisions
are not so transparent that investors have difficulty seeing
under certain risks. According to them, individuals assess
it clearly. Consequently, the decisions made will be highly
their loss and gain perspectives asymmetrically. Thus,
dependent on how the information is framed or presented.
contrary to the expected utility theory (which models the
Based on the previous experiment, Frensidy (2016) described
decision that perfectly rational agents would make), prospect
someone (suppose called Budi), in a different way by using
theory aims to describe the actual behavior of people. They
the same information on two separate groups, group A and
found that losses hurt about twice as much as gains make
B. In group A, Budi is said to be a smart, diligent, impulsive,
us feel good. That is people feel the pain of loss twice as
critical, stubborn, and jealous person, whereas, in group B,
strongly as they feel pleasure at an equal gain. The thought
Budi is described as a jealous, stubborn, critical, impulsive,
that the pain of losing is psychologically about twice as
diligent, and smart person. The same characteristics about
powerful as the pleasure of gaining is known as loss aversion.
Budi but presented in reverse order turn out to significantly
The other implication of prospect theory is people tend to
influence the groups’ assessment results. The experiment
take larger risks to avoid losses, rather than take risks to earn
results reveal that the characteristics mentioned earlier
profits. To put it another way, investors will be inclined to
have more influence than those mentioned later. Group A
be risk-averse, when coming across profits and switch to be
significantly asses Budi better than group B do. He argued
risk-takers when perceiving losses. This finding contrasts
that there are two reasons which explain such phenomena.
with the expected utility theory from Markowitz (1952)
First, one’s concentration level may decrease with the
who stated that a rational investor will exhibit consistent
increasing amount of information to be absorbed, so that
behavior, whether he/she is a risk-averse or a risk-taker
the information placed behind gets less attention. Second, under any circumstances.
first impressions usually receive more weight than the
information that comes after. These two things then lead to 2.3. Heuristic Theory
anchoring bias to occur.
The term heuristic was introduced by Tversky and
3. Hypotheses Development
Kahneman (1974) who described that the decisions made
amid complexities and conditions of uncertainty are mostly
3.1. Anchoring Bias and Investment Decisions
based on the beliefs concerning the likelihood of uncertain
events. Uncertainty in events is uncertainty regarding
According to Tversky and Kahneman (1974), anchoring
either the occurrence of an event. These beliefs then form
bias occurs when people rely too much on pre-existing
a heuristic way of thinking, by which people tend to use
information or the first information they find when
rules of thumb to simplify the decision-making processes.
making decisions (anchor). Then, adjustment is made on
This view was strengthened by De Bondt et al. (2008) that
such perception. Investors who are affected by this bias
individuals (investors) have a bias in their belief that will
tend to underlie their investment decisions on one certain
affect how they think and make decisions. Fromlet (2001)
information, regardless of whether the information is first
defined heuristics as “the use of experience and practical
acquired, or it is the only information available which made 1234
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240
people highly rely on it. Ackert and Deaves (2009) defined
(2016) is the assumption that past performance is the best
overconfidence as the tendency of a person to overestimate
indicator to predict future performance. In other words,
knowledge, ability, and the accuracy of the information
investors often believe that past rates of return represent
that an investor possesses, or the tendency to become too
future expected return. If a company announces successive
optimistic about the future and ability. Such an investor must
profit increases, investors will assume that it will continue to
be wary of anchoring bias. Despite the different information
rise and consider this company a good company, which means
available, people tend to be inclined to the first-owned
good investment. Therefore, investors expect higher returns
information in making a decision.
for past winners’ stocks and
use this trend as a stereotype for
Many investors in the capital market experience
future stock movements (Lakonishok et al., 1994; Ackert &
anchoring bias that most of them continue to remember the
Deaves, 2010). Thus, based on the explanations and reviews
buying price of shares in their portfolio. The selling decision
on earlier studies, a hypothesis is proposed as follows:
is frequently based on the buying price as the reference
point. Investors decide to sell their shares sooner when the
H2: Representativeness bias will affect investment
price is above the reference point. Besides buying price, the decisions.
highest price of shares that has ever been achieved during a
certain period also frequently become reference price. Yet,
3.3. Loss Aversion and Investment Decisions
many investors are not willing to cut loss cause they refer to
such reference prices (Frensidy, 2016). Thus, a hypothesis is
According to Pompian (2006), loss aversion is the proposed as follows:
tendency to prefer avoiding losses to acquiring equivalent
gains. Loss aversion is a tendency where investors are so
H1: Anchoring bias will affect investment decisions.
fearful of losses that they focus on trying to avoid a loss
more so than on making gains. The more one experiences
3.2. Representativeness Bias and
losses, the more likely they are to become prone to loss Investment Decisions
aversion. Research on loss aversion shows that investors
feel the pain of a loss more than twice as strongly as they
Representativeness bias is someone’s tendency to make
feel the enjoyment of making a profit. The concept of loss
decisions based on certain stereotypes or prior knowledge
aversion has emerged as an implication of prospect theory
or experiences. Representativeness bias happens when
that investors are not risk-averse, but loss averse. Such a
people make decisions only by limited observations to
thing occurs because the psychological impacts of losses are
acquire information from the surrounding environment
greater than those of profits. To put it another way, investors
and ignore other information (Baker & Nofsinger, 2002;
tend to feel more stressed by potential losses in comparison
Ritter, 2003; Shefrin, 2000). Representativeness bias tends
to potential gains with an equivalent value. Therefore, they
to lead investors to overreact during processing information
will be more prudent in investment to reduce the risk of
in making decisions (Kahneman & Riepe, 1998). It is
losses (Barberis & Thaler, 2003).
supported by the findings of Franses (2007) and Marsden
et al. (2008) who revealed that the representativeness bias
H3: Loss aversion bias will affect investment decisions.
can cause over-reaction behavior to occur as reflected in the
stock prices. On the other hand, investors who are affected
3.4. Overconfidence Bias and
by this bias can also ignore or not pay close attention to Investment Decisions
important events that may happen in the future. Hence, they
do not protect themselves from such unexpected events
Another behavioral bias that is also often found among (Yoong, 2010).
investors is overconfidence. Overconfidence bias means
Representativeness bias can lead someone to make a
that the individual is outrightly confident of his decisions
wrong conclusion. Higher-priced products are often decided
and he overestimates or exaggerates his ability to perform a
with higher quality than lower-priced ones, although there
task. Decision-makers incline to overestimate the knowledge
is a likelihood that prices do not always reflect quality.
and information that they possessed, also ignore the public
Chen et al. (2007) explained that the common stereotypes
information available. The investors with overconfidence
in the capital market are investors tend to interpret the
bias override models and data because they convince
good characteristics of a company, such as product quality,
themselves that they know better. They may not always know
reliable managers, and high growth as the characteristics of
better, and by ignoring the early signs of potential damage,
the company that has a worthwhile investment.
they cause themselves more harm than good. (Lichtenstein
The other error of representativeness bias that often
& Fischhoff, 1977). Baker and Nofsinger (2002) defined
occurs in the financial market as described by Frensidy
overconfidence as a form of excessive self-confidence that
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240 1235
the information owned can be utilized properly because of
3.5. Optimism Bias and Investment Decisions
having good analytical skills. In fact, this is only an illusion
of belief, due to lack of experience and having a shortcoming
Optimism bias often relates to overconfidence bias.
in interpreting available information.
Nofsinger (2005) explained that both overconfidence and
In short, overconfidence is a condition in which investors
optimism biases are caused by the same psychological
believe and consider their abilities are above the average of
factors – an illusion of knowledge and illusion of control.
other investors and have an unrealistic level of self-evaluation
The former is a condition in which an individual feel highly
(Odean, 1999; Pompian, 2006). Frischhoff et al. (1977)
confident with the information they possessed. It affects his/
asserted that in an uncertain world of investment, investors
her belief in the chance level of success that may be achieved.
are inclined to make overconfident decisions. Those who are
Such belief then leads to perceived control over the results
overconfident usually think that they know more than they
to be gained (the illusion of control). The illusion of control
do, so they tend to believe that they are better or smarter than
is the tendency for people to overestimate their ability to
others (Shiller, 2000). When the number of market participants
control events; for example, it occurs when someone feels a
who are overconfident is large, then the aggregate reactions
sense of control over outcomes that they demonstrably do not
that occur in the market will be far from being ration8al.
influence. Thus, control illusions are defined as a situation in
According to Frensidy (2016), an individual’s inclination
which people frequently believe that they have influenced
to be overconfident may be caused by two things. First,
the results obtained from an uncontrolled event. When an
except for those who are depressed, everyone positively
illusion of knowledge and illusion of control evolve further,
judges themselves. Second, psychologically, people want
there will appear as excessive optimism (Shefrin, 2007).
to control the situation and their surroundings and believe
Shefrin (2007) defined optimism bias as one who inclines
that they can do that. Further, he revealed that there are four
to overestimate success (the likelihood of obtaining results
financial implications of this bias. First, investors can take the
as desired) and underestimating the risk of failure. Further,
wrong position in buying and selling shares because they fail
optimism bias is an investor’s expectation or belief that their
to realize that they do not have the advantage of information
portfolio performance will always generate a positive return
or analysis. Second, investors are inclined to trade more
(Hoffmann et al., 2013). The important thing from this bias is that
frequently which results in higher transaction costs. Third,
there is a likelihood that investors make investment decisions
overconfident investors are inclined to set prediction
excessively. The higher level of optimism bias that occurred,
intervals that are too narrow. Finally, overconfident investors
the higher investor’s expectation of their portfolio performance.
will be surprised more often than expected.
This positive expectation then spurs them to increase the
A handful of studies have been conducted to investigate
frequency and volume of trading, even though there is a high
the influence of overconfidence bias on the financial market.
probability that the actual may deviate from the expectations
Overconfidence behavior unconsciously influences investors
(Pompian, 2006). Khan et al., (2017) and Ullah et al. (2017)
to do excessive trading (Benos, 1998; Daniel et al., 1998;
found a relationship between past portfolio yields and the level
Graham et al., 2005; Odean, 1999; Pompian, 2006; Toma,
of investor optimism that had an impact on investment decisions.
2015; Ullah et al., 2017). Besides affecting the frequency
of transactions, overconfidence bias also affects trading
H5: Optimism bias will affect investment decisions.
volume. The higher the overconfident level, the greater the
volume traded (Statman et al., 2003). These results indicate a
3.6. Herding Behavior and Investment Decisions
positive influence of overconfidence bias towards investment
decision making. Besides, Bakar and Yi (2016) also revealed
Banerjee (1992) and Hirshleifer and Teoh (2003) explained
that the level of overconfidence that occurs also depends on
herding behavior as a people behavior that tends to follow the individual gender.
actions of other people rather than following their owned-
Those research findings are in line with Pompian (2006)
beliefs or owned-information in the making decision. This
who found that the belief of overconfidence in skills can
behavior is considered irrational behavior as investors decide
cause mistakes in decision making that make investors
based on other’s decisions in the market (Altman, 2012).
trade excessively. The overconfident investor also tends to
Herding behavior is often found among investors in emerging
overestimate investment returns and underestimate risks. If
markets and mostly occurred during market stress situations
the actual return is lower than the expected return, they will
(Rahayu et al., 2020). According to Humra (2014), herding
associate it with an unfortunate condition (Miller, 1975).
behavior occurs when a group of investors make investment
Overall, these various factors will have a positive impact on
decisions based on collective information from a group of investment decisions.
investors and ignore other information. As a result, when
the group majority makes a wrong decision, it will turn to
H4: Overconfidence bias will affect investment decisions.
significant market price deviations. 1236
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240
The finding of Chang et al. (2000) showed that herding
analyzed based on validity and reliability test to validate the
practices are more prevalent in developing countries, which
questionnaires using Bivariate Pearson correlation (Pearson
then is supported by Chiang and Zheng (2010) and Zheng Product Moment).
et al. (2017) concerning the herding practices in Asian stock
exchanges (China, South Korea, Singapore, Malaysia, and r N XY ( X)( Y )
Indonesia). Herd mentality bias refers to investors’ tendency xy [{N X2 ( X)2 }{ N Y2 ( Y)2 }]
to follow and copy what other investors are doing. They are
largely influenced by emotion and instinct, rather than by r
their own independent analysis. In Indonesia, the research
xy = Pearson Correlation Coefficient
findings related to herding behavior are still contradictive, X
= Scores for each question or statement item
even though they are tested by the same methods. Sari Y
= Scores for total question items or statements
(2012), and Purba and Faradynawati (2012) revealed there
X = total scores in X distribution
had been herding practices in Indonesia, whereas Narasanto
Y = total scores in Y distribution
(2012) did not find herding practices. Furthermore, Bowe
X 2 = total squares of each X score
and Domuta (2004), using the Lakonishok et al. (1992)
method, found that herding behavior in the Indonesian Stock
Y 2 = total squares of each Y score
Exchange was mostly dominated by foreign investors. N = total subjects
H6: Herding behavior will affect investment decisions.
The reliability of items that test the degree of stability,
consistency, predictive power, and accuracy are measured 4. Methodology
based on the Cronbach alpha formula:
The population of this study is investors over the age S2 2 r S1
of 17 years and based in Yogyakarta and based on random α = K K 1 S2
sampling that results in 165 respondents. To determine the x
sample size, we use the Slovin formula. It provides the
sample size (n) using the known population size (N) and α
= Cronbach’ alpha reliability coefficient
the acceptable error value (e). Slovin’s formula gives the K = total question items tested
researcher an idea of how large the sample size needs to be
s2 = total item score variants
to ensure a reasonable accuracy of results. 1
S X 2 = Variance of test scores (all items) Z2 n
Then, One sample t-test is used to test the effect of anchoring 2 4 Moe
bias, representativeness bias, loss aversion, overconfidence
bias, optimism bias, and herding behavior on investor decisions. Z
= Level of confidence, this study uses a 95%
The formula for one-sample t-test is used as follows: confidence level
Moe = The maximum tolerable error rate is 8% X n = sample size Z x S n
We collect the data using questionnaires based on the
5-Likert scale, with 1 = Strongly Disagree; 2 = Disagree;
5. Results and Discussion
3 = Neutral; 4 = Agree; and 5 = Strongly Agree. We define
only score 4 and 5 that considered investor decisions
Table 1 displays the information about the characteristics
are influenced by behavioral bias, otherwise, it does not
of the respondents. The results of the Pearson Bivariate
(Altamimi, 2006). The total number of questions from all
correlation test on the total questionnaire items indicate that
variables are 42 questions − 5 questions for anchoring bias,
all of them are valid based on the r-value of each item which
8 questions for representativeness bias, 5 questions for loss
is greater than the value of the r-table. The Cronbach’s alpha
aversion, 7 questions for overconfidence bias, 7 questions
score also shows similar results where the value for each item
for optimism bias, and 4 questions for herding behavior.
is greater than 0.6. The score for each variable - anchoring
Six independent variables are used in the model -anchoring
bias, representativeness bias, loss aversion, over-evidence
bias, representativeness bias, loss aversion, overconfidence
bias, optimism bias, and herding behavior are 0.61, 0.77,
bias, optimism bias, and herding behavior, while the
0.67, 0.86, 0.78, and 0.75 respectively. The Kolmogorov-
dependent variable is investment decisions. The data is first
Smirnov test is used to check the normality and it shows that
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240 1237
Table 1: Sample Descriptive
5.2. Representativeness Bias and Investment Decisions Number of Demographic Factors Percentage Respondents
The result of the second hypothesis testing indicates that
representativeness bias has a significant influence on invest- Gender Male 94 57%
ment decisions. Investors tend to make decisions simply based Female 71 43%
on limited information from the surroundings and ignore other Level of Postgraduate 5 3%
information or the important events that may happen in the Education Bachelor’s 82 49.7%
future. Hence, they are less prepared for unexpected events degree
or information. They also consider that past performance as
the best indicator to predict future performance. The repre- Associate 5 3% degree
sentativeness bias further supports the notion that people fail
to properly calculate and utilize probability in their decisions. Senior high 73 44.2%
The research finding is consistent with Toma (2015), Vijaya school
(2016), and Virigineni and Bhaskara (2017). Investment Stocks 141 85.5% selected Bonds 4 2.4%
5.3. Loss Aversion and Investment Decisions Mutual funds 15 9.1%
The third hypothesis result shows that investors are Others 5 3%
prudent in deciding to buy or sell the shares to avoid the loss.
They focus more on avoiding the losses instead of gaining
Table 2: The Result of One-Sample T-Test
higher profits. Very often, stocks are bought without much
research. So, once the stock price goes up, investors fear that Variables Mean Significance Result
it may go down as fast as it went up. Such thinking makes
them sell the stocks too soon. Then come instances where Anchoring bias 3.84 0.000 H0 rejected
the stock price has gone down after an investor has bought Representativeness bias 3.78 0.000 H0 rejected
it. This tends to happen when the primary reason for buying Loss aversion 4.09 0.000 H0 rejected
the stock was a recent upsurge. Hence, when the value of Overconfidence bias 3.27 0.000 H0 rejected
their portfolio investment decreases, they prefer to retain it
as they hope that it would increase to the previous price in Optimism bias 3.95 0.000 H0 rejected
the future. On the other hand, they tend to sell their stock so Herding behavior 3.30 0.000 H0 rejected
early when the investment value increases. These findings
support the concept of disposition effect and are in line with
the data is distributed normally. The results of one sample
that of Luong and Doan (2011), Ngoc (2014), Khan (2017),
t-test indicate that the average value of each variable is
Kimeu et al. (2016), and Rekik and Boujelbene (2013).
greater than 3.00 and all of the hypotheses are significant at
1% alpha. It indicates that most of the investors in Yogyakarta
5.4. Overconfidence Bias and
tend to be affected by anchoring bias, representativeness Investment Decisions
bias, loss aversion, overconfidence bias, optimism bias, and
herding behavior in making investment decisions.
We also find in this study that overconfidence bias has a
significant influence on investment decisions. Considering
5.1. Anchoring Bias and Investment Decisions
that the major respondents are college students, there is a
high level of probability that they tend to have a higher level
The result of the first hypothesis suggests that there is an
of enthusiasm and motivation to get into the investment
inclination of the investors to sell the stocks based on buying
world. However, enthusiasm and motivation themselves are
price as the reference price. The investors make quick decisions
not enough to be a good investor. They need to develop more
to sell their shares when the selling price exceeds the buying
investment skills and broaden the knowledge of investment
price. Besides buying price, the highest price that was achieved
which they do not have enough. Stock investment is a long-
during a certain period also becomes the reference price.
term investment that has the highest risk compared with
Besides, investors decide to buy stocks based on the past stocks’
other types of investments, such as mutual funds or bonds.
performance which means the investors overestimate their
This is worth noting for young investors who are very
own opinions and expertise. This finding is in line with that of
vulnerable to overconfidence bias. This empirical finding is
Frensidy (2016), Vijaya (2014), Rekik and Boujelbene (2013),
consistent with Toma (2015), Bakar and Yi (2016), and Ullah
Luong and Doan (2011), and Masomi and Ghayekhloo (2010). et al. (2017). 1238
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240
5.5. Optimism Bias and Investment Decisions
Review_Cambridge_Vol5No_2225-232_2006/links/00b495 3ab9c4e8ba85000000.pdf
The result of the fifth hypothesis also describes that
Altman, M. (2012). Behavioral economics for dummies.
optimism bias affects investment decisions significantly.
Mississauga: John Wiley & Sons.
The optimism bias is an expectation or belief that the
Asri, M. (2013). Behavioral finance. Yogyakarta: BPFE-
future performance will always better than the past return Yogyakarta.
(Hoffmann et al., 2013). As the respondents are dominated
by the young investors, who are highly susceptible to
Bakar, S., & Yi, A. C. (2016). The impact of psychological factors
on investors’ decision-making in the Malaysian stock market:
be overconfident, it will be followed by a high level of
a case of Klang Valley and Pahang. Procedia Economics
optimism. Overconfidence and optimism biases are caused
and Finance, 35, 319–328. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2212-
by the illusion of knowledge and illusion of control. The 5671(16)00040-X
illusion of knowledge is a condition where a person feels
Baker, H. K., & Nofsinger J. R. (2002). Psychological bias of
very confident about the information he/she has so that it
investors. Financial Services Review. 11(2), 97–116. https://
has an impact on his/her belief in their chance of success. scinapse.io/papers/172315172
It supports by the empirical finding of Khan et al. (2017),
Fatima and Waqas (2016), and Ullah et al. (2017).
Banerjee, A. V. (1992). A simple model of herd behavior. The
Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(3), 797–817. https://doi. org/10.2307/2118364
5.6. Herding Behavior and Investment Decisions
Barberis, N., & Thaler, R. (2003). A survey of behavioral finance.
This study also found that herding behavior has a
Handbook of the Economics of Finance, 25(2), 1053–1128.
significant influence on investment decisions. It indicates
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1574-0102(03)01027-6
that investors tend to rely on collective information from
Benos, A. V. (1998). Aggressiveness and survival of overconfident
other investors rather than personal information. In this
traders. Journal of Financial Markets, 1(3–4), 353-383. https://
respect, the investors react impulsively to the changes
doi.org/10.1016/S1386-4181(97)00010-4
found in others’ decisions as they prefer others’ investment
Bowe, M., & Domuta, D. (2004). Investor herding during the
choices to their own choices. They put little attention on the
financial crisis: A clinical study of the Jakarta Stock Exchange.
company’s prospects and believe more about what others
Pacific-Basin Finance Journal, 12(4), 387–418. https://doi.
decide in the market. This finding is in line with the studies
org/10.1016/j.pacfin.2003.09.003
of Vijaya (2014), Ngoc (2014), Ranjbar et al. (2014), and
Chang, E. C., Cheng, J. W., & Khorana, A. (2000). An examination Kumar and Goyal (2015).
of herd behavior in equity markets. International Perspective
Journal of Banking and Finance
, 24, 1651–1679. https://doi.
org/10.1016/S0378-4266(99)00096-5 6. Conclusion
Chen, G., Kim, K. A., & Nofsinger, J. R. (2007). Trading
Our research findings suggest that anchoring bias,
performance, disposition effect, overconfidence, represent-
representativeness bias, loss aversion, overconfidence bias,
ativeness bias, and experience of emerging market investors.
optimism bias, and herding behavior affect significantly the
Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20(4), 425–451.
https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.957504
investor’s decisions. There are a few opportunities for much
more comprehensive research on investors’ behavioral biases.
Chiang, T. C., & Zheng, D. (2010). An empirical analysis of
herd behavior in global stock markets. Journal of Banking
and Finance
, 34, 1911–1921. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. References jbankfin.2009.12.014
Ackert, L. F., & Deaves R. (2010). Behavioral finance psychology,
Daniel, K., Hirshleifer, D., & Subrahmanyam, A. (1998). Investor
decision making, and markets. Cincinnati, OH: South-Western,
psychology and security market under and overreaction. Journal Cengage Learning.
of Finance, 53(6), 1839–1885. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022- 1082.00077
Al-Mansour, B. Y. (2020). Cryptocurrency market: Behavioral
finance perspective. Journal of Asian Finance, Economics, and
De Bondt, W. F. M., & Thaler, R. (1985). Does the stock market
Business, 7(12), 159–165. https://doi.org/10.13106/jafeb.2020.
overreact? Journal of Finance, 40(3), 793–805. https://doi. vol7.no12.159 org/10.2307/2327804
Altamimi, H. A. H. (2006). Factors influencing individual investor
De Bondt, W., Muradoglu, G., Shefrin, S., & Staikouras, S. K.
behavior: An empirical study of the UAE financial market. The
(2008). Behavioral finance: Quo Vadis? Journal of Applied
Business Review. 5(2), 225–232. https://www.researchgate.net/ Finance 18
, (2), 1–15. https://ssrn.com/abstract=269814
profile/Hussein_Al-Tamimi/publication/257936341_Factors_
Fama, E. F. (1970). Efficient capital markets: A review on theory
Influencing_Individual_Investor_Behaviour_An_Empirical_
and empirical work. Journal of Finance, 25(2), 383–417.
study_of_the_UAE_Financial_MarketsThe_Business_
https://doi.org/10.2307/2325486
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240 1239
Fatima, N., & Waqas, M. (2016). Impact of optimistic bias and
Management & Administration, 1(1), 17–28. http://www.
availability bias on investment decision making with the
impactjournals.us/download/archives/1-82-1502108083-3.
moderating role of financial literacy. First International
Khan, M. T., Chong, L. L., & Tan, S. H. (2017). Perception of past
Conference of Indigenous Resource Management challenges
portfolio returns, optimism, and financial decisions. Review of
and Opportunities at University of Management Sciences and
Behavioral Finance, 9(1), 79–98. https://doi.org/10.1108/RBF-
Information Technology Kotli, Azaad Kashmir 1415 April 02-2016-0005 2016 (pp. 1–28).
Kimeu, C. N., Anyago. W., & Rotich, G. (2016). Behavioural
Franses, P. H. (2007). Experts adjusting model-based forecast and
factors influencing investment decisions among individual
the law of small numbers (Report No. EI 2007-42). Econometric
investors in Nairobi Securities Exchange. The Strategic Journal
Institute, Erasmus University Rotterdam. https://repub.eur.nl/
of Business & Change Management, 3(4), 1243–1258. http:// pub/10563
www.strategicjournals.com/index.php/journal/article/view/377
Frensidy, B. (2016). Agile and tactical in the capital market:
Kumar, S., & Goyal, N. (2015). Behavioral biases in investment
Armed with behavioral finance. Jakarta: Salemba Empat.
decision making; A systematic literature review. Qualitative
Frischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1977). Knowing
Research in Financial Markets, 7(1), 88–108.
with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Lakonishok, J., Shleifer, A., & Vishny, R. W. (1992). The impact
of institutional trading on, stock price. Journal of Financial
Performance, 3(4), 552–564. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096- 1523.3.4.552
Economics, 32(1), 23–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304- 405X(92)90023-Q
Fromlet, H. (2001). Behavioral finance-theory and practical
Lakonishok, J., Shleifer, A., & Vishny, R. W. (1994). Contrarian
application. Business Economics, 36(3), 63–69. https://doi.
investment, extrapolation, and risk. org/10.2139/ssrn.2329573
Journal of Finance, (5), 49
1541–1578. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1994.tb04772
Graham, J. R., Harvey, C. R., & Huang, H. (2005). Investor
competence, trading frequency, and home bias (NBER
Lichtenstein, S., & Fischhoff, B. (1977). Do those who know more
Working Paper 11426). National Bureau of Economic
also know more about how much they know? Organizational
Research https://www.nber.org/system/files/working_papers/
Behavior and Human Performance. 20(2), 159–83. https://doi. w11426/w11426.pdf org/10.1177/002224379202900304
Hirschey, M., & Nofsinger R. J. (2008). Investment: Analysis and
Lintner, J. (1965). Security prices, risk, and maximum gains from
behavioral. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill/Irwin.
diversification. Journal of Finance, 20(4), 587–615. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1965.tb02930
Hirshleifer, D., & Teoh, S. H. (2003). Herd behavior and cascading
in capital markets: A review and synthesis. European Financial
Luong, L. P., & Doan, T. T. H. (2011). Behavioral individual Management 9 , (1), 25–66.
investors’ decision making and performance a survey at the
Ho Chi Minh stock exchange
[Master Thesis, Umeå School of
Hoffmann, A. O., Post, T., & Pennings M. E. (2013). Individual
Business]. http://umu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:423263/
investor perception and behavior during the financial crisis. FULLTEXT02.pdf
Journal of Banking and Finance, 37(1), 60–74. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jbankfin.2012.08.007
Markowitz, H. (1952). Portfolio selection. Journal of Finance,
7(1), 77–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1952.tb01525
Humra, Y. (2014). Behavioral finance: An introduction to the
principles governing investor behavior in stock markets.
Marsden, A., Veeraraghavan, M., & Ye. M. (2008). Heuristics of
International Journal of Financial Management, 5(2), 23–30.
representativeness, anchoring, and adjustment, and leniency:
http://iaset.us/download/archives/2-35-1456481625-3.%20
impact on earnings forecasts by Australian analysts. Quarterly Abstract.pdf
Journal of Finance and Accounting, 47(2), 83–102. http://hdl. handle.net/2292/8518
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis
of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–291. https://
Masomi, S. R., & Ghayekhloo, S. (2010). Consequences of human doi.org/10.2307/1914185
behaviors’ in economic: The effects of behavioral factors
in investment decision making at Teheran Stock Exchange.
Kahneman, D., & Riepe, M. W. (1998). Aspects of investor
International Conference on Business and Economics Research,
psychology: Beliefs, preferences, and biases investment
1, 1–4. http://ipedr.com/vol1/50-B10068.pdf
advisors should know about. Journal of Portfolio Management,
24(4), 52–65. https://doi.org/10.3905/jpm.1998.409643
Miller, D. T. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction.
Kartini, K., & Nuris, F. N. (2015). The influence of illusions of
Psychological Bulletin, (2), 82
213–225. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076486
control, overconfidence, and emotional biases on investment
decisions. Jurnal Inovasi dan Kewirausahaan, 4(2), 115–123.
Mossin, J. (1969). Security pricing and investment criteria in
https://doi.org/10.20885/ajie.vol4.iss2.art6
competitive markets. American Economic Review, (5), 59
749–756. https://www.jstor.org/stable/1810673
Khan, M. U. (2017). Impact of availability bias and loss aversion
bias on investment decision making, moderating role of risk
Narasanto, I. T. (2012). Herding behavior an experience
perception. Journal of Modern Developments in General
in Indonesian stock market (No, 5589) [Master Thesis, 1240
Kartini KARTINI, Katiya NAHDA / Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business Vol 8 No 3 (2021) 1231–1240
Perpustakaan Universitas Gadjah Mada, Yogyakarata] http://
Forecasting, 18(3), 375–382. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-
etd.repository.ugm.ac.id/home/detail_pencarian/55899 2070(02)00021-3
Ngoc, L. T. B. (2014). The behavior pattern of individual investors
Shefrin, H. (2007). Behavioral corporate finance: Decision that
in the stock market. International Journal of Business and
creates value. McGraw-Hill/Irwin Management 9
, (1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.5539/ijbm.v9n1p1
Shiller, R. J. (1987). Investor behavior in the October 1987
Nofsinger, J. R. (2005). Social mood and financial economics.
stock market crash: Survey evidence (Working Paper
Journal of Behavioral Finance, 6(3), 144–160. https://doi.
2446). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.
org/10.1207/s15427579jpfm0603_4 org/10.3386/w2446
Odean, T. (1999). Do investors trade too much? American Economic
Shiller, R. J. (2000). Irrational exuberance. Princeton, NJ: Review 89
, , 1279–98. https://doi.org/10.2139/SSRN.94143 Princeton University Press.
Olsen. R. A. (1998). Behavioral finance and its implication for
Statman, M., Thorley, S., & Vorkink, K. (2003). Investor
stock price volatility. Financial Analyst Journal, (2), 52 10–18.
overconfidence and trading volume. Review of Financial
https://doi.org/10.2469/faj.v54.n2.2161
Studies, 19(4), 1531–1565. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.168472
Pompian, M. M. (2006). Behavioral finance and wealth
Subash, R. (2012). Role of behavioral finance in portfolio
management. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
investment decision: Evidence from India [Master Thesis,
Charles University in Prague]. https://is.cuni.cz/webapps/zzp/
Purba, A. V., & Faradynawati, A. A. (2012). An examination of herd detail/110165/?lang=en
behavior in the Indonesian Stock Market. Indonesian Capital
Market Review
, 4(1), 1–10. https://doi/org/10.21002/icmr.v4i1.985
Toma, F. M. (2015). Behavioral biases of the investment decisions
of Romanian investors on the Bucharest Stock Exchange.
Rahayu, S., Rohman, A., & Harto, P. (2020). Herding behavior
model in investment decision on Emerging Markets:
Procedia Economics and Finance, 32, 200–207. https://doi.
org/10.1016/S2212-5671(15)01383-0
Experimental in Indonesia. Journal of Asian Finance,
Economics, and Business, 8
(1), 53–59. https://doi.
Treynor. J. L. (1961). Market value, time, and risk [Unpublished
org/10.13106/jafeb.2021.vol8.no1.053
manuscript]. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2600356
Ranjbar, M. H., Abedini, B., & Jamali, M. (2014). Analyzing the
Tversky, A., & Kahneman D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty:
effective behavioral factors on the investors’ performance
Heuristics and biases. Science, New Series, 185(4157),
in Tehran Stock Exchange (TSE). International Journal of 1124–1131.
Technical Research and Applications, 2(8), 2320–8163.
Ullah, I., Ullah. A., & Rehman, N. U. (2017). Impact of
Rekik, Y. M., & Boujelbene, Y. (2013). Determinants of individual
overconfidence and optimism on investment decision.
investors’ behaviors: Evidence from Tunisian Stock Market.
International Journal of Information, Business, and
IOSR Journal of Business and Management, 8(2), 109–119.
Management, 9(2), 231–243. http://ejournal.ukrida.ac.id/ojs/
https://doi.org/10.9790/487X-082109119
index.php/Akun/article/download/1802/1821/
Ritter, J. R. (2003). Behavioral finance. Pacific-Basin Finance
Vijaya, E. (2014). An empirical analysis of influential factors on
Journal, 11(4), 429–437. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0927-
investment behavior of retail investors’ in the Indian Stock 538X(03)00048-9
Market: A behavioral perspective. International Journal in
Sari, A. M. (2012). Herding behavior in Indonesia and the impact of
Management and Social Science, 2(12), 296–308. https://
www.indianjournals.com/ijor.aspx?target=ijor:ijmss&volume=
the global stock market. Master Y
Thesis. ogyakarta: FEB UGM 2&issue=12&article=027
Sha, N., & Ismail, M. Y. (2020). Behavioral investor types and
Vijaya, E. (2016). An empirical analysis on the behavioral
financial market players in Oman. Journal of Asian Finance,
pattern of Indian retail equity investors. Journal of Resources
Economics, and Business, 8(1), 285–294. https://doi.
Development and Management, 16, 103–112. https://core.
org/10.13106/jafeb.2021.vol8.no1.285
ac.uk/download/pdf/234696209.pdf
Sharpe, W. F. (1964). Capital asset prices: A theory of market
Virigineni, M., & Bhaskara, R. (2017). Contemporary developments
equilibrium under the condition of risk. Journal of Finance, 19(3),
in behavioral finance. International Journal of Economics and
425–442. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1964.tb02865
Financial, 7(1), 448–459. https://www.econjournals.com/
Shefrin, H., & Statman, M. (1985). The disposition to sell winners
index.php/ijefi/article/view/3809
too early and riding losers too long. Journal of Finance, (3), 40
Yoong, J. (2010). Making financial education more effective:
777–790. https://doi.org/10.2307/2327802
lessons from behavioral economics (Working paper Session II).
Shefrin, H. (2000). Beyond greed and fear: Understanding
Behavioral Economics and Financial Education.
behavioral finance and the psychology of investing. New York,
Zheng, D., Li. H., & Chiang, T. C. (2017). Herding with NY: Oxford University Press.
industries: Evidence from Asian stock markets. International
Shefrin H. (2002). Behavioral decision making, forecasting,
Review of Economics and Finance, 51, 487–509. https://doi.
game theory, and role-play. International Journal of org/10.1016/j.iref.2017.07.005